-
>> The field of
neuroscience is
-
an incredibly
exciting field
-
to be working in,
-
to be studying, and
to be teaching.
-
Many people think
of the field as
-
being about 50 years old.
-
It was in the
mid 1960s that
-
the first
universities began to
-
have academic departments
-
devoted to neuroscience,
-
graduate programs
devoted to neuroscience.
-
In 1969, the Society for
-
Neuroscience was founded.
-
And now 50 years later,
-
it has 35,000 members and
-
affiliate organizations in
-
almost 100 countries.
-
And much of what
we're going to be
-
talking about in
the course to come
-
this semester is
going to focus
-
on those last 50 years
-
where the field
has exploded
-
year by year and
decade by decade.
-
But these 50 years of
-
neuroscience didn't
start from scratch.
-
In fact, they
built on 2,000
-
years of work by
philosophers,
-
and doctors, and
scientists trying
-
to understand how
the brain worked.
-
So today, I want
to take you on
-
a very brief
15-minute tour of
-
those 2,000 years of
-
prehistory of
neuroscience.
-
So buckle your seat belts
-
and be prepared
to find out
-
where and how
did the study of
-
how does the brain
work begins.
-
This history
will cover the
-
first 2,000 or so years of
-
the field from about
400 BC until the 1950s.
-
And we'll focus
on three classes
-
of questions which
have predominated
-
over these last two
millennia as people
-
struggle to understand
-
how does the brain work.
-
These three classes
of questions
-
begin with philosophy,
functional anatomy,
-
and lastly, cellular
mechanisms.
-
And let me tell you
some of the questions
-
that each of these
areas have asked.
-
In philosophy,
the questions
-
that are predominated
have been,
-
what is the
brain good for?
-
Is the mind distinct
-
from mechanisms
of the brain?
-
Functional anatomy
has asked a number of
-
different questions
that try to localize
-
where in the brain
different functions occur.
-
Some of these
questions are,
-
what is the
functional role of
-
brain subregions
and nerves?
-
How can we understand and
-
treat different mental
health disorders?
-
Can localized brain
lesions tell us about
-
the functional
organization of the brain?
-
How is memory organized,
-
and how and where are
-
learned reflexes formed?
-
The third class
of questions
-
are about cellular
mechanisms,
-
questions such as,
-
what is the structure
of individual neurons?
-
What is the function of
neurons and synapses?
-
And how do neurons use
-
electrical signals
to communicate?
-
So these three classes of
-
questions have
emerged over
-
the last two millennia in
-
the following
approximate timeline.
-
So the philosophical
questions
-
began early on when there
-
wasn't a lot
of technology,
-
there wasn't a
lot of data from
-
400 BC to what
is the brain
-
good for to the
early 1600s
-
when philosophers asked,
-
is the mind distinct from
-
the mechanisms
of the brain?
-
Questions about
functional anatomy
-
emerge in the
middle period,
-
from the early BC,
-
even up through
the early 1900s,
-
asking the variety of
-
questions about how do we
-
localize function to
-
specific parts
of the anatomy.
-
And lastly, with
the development of
-
newer technologies
for looking at and
-
measuring and
analyzing the brain,
-
we see the cellular
mechanisms
-
have emerged
later in history.
-
So now you understand
the timeline,
-
there are going to be
-
10 questions that I'm
going to focus on
-
starting in 400 BC
-
and taking us
through the 1950s.
-
>> [MUSIC]
-
>> The first question
was back in Greece,
-
where Hippocrates
and Aristotle
-
argued about the question,
-
what is the brain for?
-
What is the
brain good for?
-
Now, Hippocrates
believed it
-
was the seat of
intelligence,
-
it was key for sensation
and perception,
-
and it was what was
disrupted in epilepsy.
-
Three things which
have still held
-
true even all
these years later.
-
Aristotle thought
the brain
-
was a cooling mechanism,
-
something like a
radiator for the blood,
-
and he believed the
heart, not the brain,
-
was the source of
-
rationality and
intelligence.
-
Well, clearly, these
two ideas turned out to
-
be wrong, but
to his credit,
-
Aristotle got many
other things right
-
about psychology and
philosophy in the brain,
-
including the fundamental
principles of
-
learning and the
essential role
-
of experience
in knowledge,
-
what we call empiricism.
-
Moving ahead in
time, Galen,
-
often called also
Claudius Galenus, asked,
-
what is the
functional role of
-
the brain subregions
and nerves?
-
Galen was a physician,
-
and his main day
job was working for
-
the Romans fixing
gladiators
-
who had been damaged
after battle.
-
He believed temperament
and bodily functions
-
were controlled
by the brain,
-
and he dissected
sheep, monkeys,
-
and dogs to learn more
about the body because
-
Roman law at that
time forbade
-
autopsy on humans.
-
Now, from these
autopsies that he did,
-
these dissections
on animals,
-
he made a number of
conclusions that
-
have still held
true today.
-
He concluded that the
hard cerebellum in
-
the back of the brain was
-
for muscle control,
-
while the softer cerebrum
around the top and
-
around the shell of
-
the brain was
for the senses,
-
and that we know
to be true.
-
He dissected nerves
and believed that
-
each nerve pathway
controlled
-
a different set
of muscles,
-
again, something
that still
-
has held true to this day.
-
Moving across to another
part of the world,
-
there was a renaissance
in about 1,000 AD of
-
Muslim science
and medicine that
-
spanned not only Iberia,
-
Southern Europe,
but we now call
-
Spain and Portugal,
-
all the way to
the Middle East.
-
And this Muslim
renaissance gave us
-
many advances in science
and in medicine.
-
And it particularly
focused on the question,
-
how can we understand and
-
treat mental
health disorders?
-
And one of the great
Muslim doctors and
-
scholars was known
as Al-Zahrawi,
-
and he lived in Islamic
-
Iberia and he described
-
many surgical
treatments for
-
neurological disorders
and brain trauma.
-
And he wrote a 30-volume
encyclopedia of
-
medicine called the
Kitab al-Tasrif.
-
The surgery chapter of
-
that volume was
translated into
-
Latin and for the
next 500 years
-
became the standard text.
-
Not that anyone today
would really want
-
to participate in any
of these surgeries,
-
but at the time,
from 1000-1500 AD,
-
they were state
of the art in
-
neurological surgery. At
around the same time,
-
in Persia, Ibn-Sina,
-
also known as Avicenna
in other rings,
-
he's often viewed as
-
the father of
modern medicine,
-
and his textbook, the
Canon of Medicine,
-
was used for
hundreds of years.
-
He described numerous
psychiatric disorders,
-
including mania,
hallucinations, dementia,
-
melancholia, or what we
now call depression,
-
and even described
-
a constellation
of symptoms
-
in people that he
called Junin Mufrit,
-
which maps very closely to
-
what we view as
schizophrenia.
-
So he really
developed a lot of
-
the basic understanding
and classifications of
-
the different mental
health disorders.
-
He also identified
and named
-
the cerebellar vermis
-
and the caudate nucleus.
-
And lastly, he
posited the brain as
-
the place where reason
-
interacts with sensation.
-
And that was
really critical
-
that it wasn't
just the thinking;
-
it was the interaction
between thinking,
-
and reason, and sensation.
-
And we very much
believe today that,
-
in fact, that's where much
-
of that intersection
takes place.
-
We now go back to Europe
next to the 1600s,
-
where Rene Descartes
-
asked the philosophal
question,
-
is the mind distinct
from the brain?
-
Now, Descartes was a firm
believer in dualism,
-
the principle
that the mind and
-
body exist as
separate entities,
-
each with a different
characteristics
-
and governed by
its own laws.
-
He also believed that
the pineal gland
-
was the link between
the mind and the body.
-
The pineal gland
lies in the chest.
-
Well, clearly, these
two approaches,
-
these two ideas
haven't held up well.
-
On the other hand,
-
he did develop the idea
of the reflex arc,
-
an automatic pathway from
-
a sensory stimulus
to a motor response,
-
and he diagrammed it here
-
in a sketch which pretty
-
much captures how we see
-
reflex arcs these days
-
coming in through the eye,
-
going to the brain,
and going out
-
through the muscles
in order to point.
-
By the mid 1800s,
-
there were a
number of people,
-
Bell, Flourens,
Harlow, Broca,
-
Wernicke, who all asked,
-
can localized
brain lesions
-
tell us about
the functional
-
organization of the brain?
-
We'll go through each of
-
these five men in order.
-
Charles Bell in
Scotland, again,
-
asked this question about
-
localized brain lesions
-
and what it tells
-
us about functional
organization.
-
He discovered
through dissection
-
and vivisection that
-
the spine transmit
motor impulses
-
and receives
sensory input.
-
He was a talented artist,
-
and you can see one of
-
his engravings in the
upper left corner,
-
and he published a
very influential
-
engravings of the brain.
-
He also described a
facial paralysis,
-
which is caused by
-
a damage to a
nerve in the face,
-
and is today known as
Bell's palsy after him.
-
In France, Jean
Pierre Flourens
-
showed that localized
brain lesions in
-
rabbits and pigeons
produced specific deficits
-
in motor control,
sensation, and behavior.
-
And he made three
important linkages.
-
He linked the cerebrum
to cognition,
-
the cerebellum
to movement,
-
and the medulla to
vital bodily functions.
-
And all of these are
-
basically the substance of
-
what we believe today.
-
On the other hand,
-
he didn't get
everything right.
-
He was a fervent
creationist,
-
and he argued against
Darwin's theory
-
of evolution by
natural selection.
-
Over here in the
United States,
-
John Harlow had a patient
named Phineas Gage,
-
who was working
on the railroad
-
when his frontal lobe was
-
pierced by an iron rod
-
that you can see here
from an autopsy,
-
literally went
through his cheek,
-
through his eye,
-
into his frontal
lobe, and surprising
-
everyone, Gage survived.
-
He still could
talk, he could
-
still function
to some degree.
-
But he lost what we
think of today as
-
executive control
over his behaviors.
-
He became impulsive and
-
easily distracted
and angry.
-
And so it was
from this that
-
Phineas Gage
suggested that
-
this part of the brain,
-
the front part
of the brain
-
of Phineas Gage was
-
critical for
these kinds of
-
executive controls
over our behavior.
-
Pierre Broca in
France studied
-
12 patients who lost
-
the ability to speak but
-
were still able
to comprehend.
-
And when these
patients died
-
and he autopsied them,
-
he showed that they
all had damage
-
in the left frontal lobe,
-
an area of the
brain now known as
-
Broca's area and believed
-
and known to be
key for speech.
-
Over in Germany, Carl
Wernicke was looking at
-
some very different
patients that
-
were essentially the
inverse or the opposite.
-
He studied patients who
-
lost the ability
to comprehend,
-
but they were still
able to speak.
-
Again, on later autopsies,
-
he showed that these
people had damage
-
in the superior
temporal gyrus,
-
an area now known
as Wernicke's area,
-
and critical for
comprehension.
-
And you can see
in the diagram
-
there, a modern diagram,
-
the difference
between Broca's area
-
and Wernicke's area,
-
suggesting that
comprehension and production
-
of language are subserved
-
by different
brain regions.
-
Moving ahead to
the late 1800s
-
back in the US,
-
William James asked, how
is memory organized?
-
He taught the
first course in
-
psychology ever
given in America,
-
and he was especially
interested
-
in how we learn new habits
-
and acquire new memories.
-
And to explain the
development of
-
memories and
how one memory
-
can trigger
another memory,
-
he described a
network model
-
where he suggested
that, at one event,
-
a dinner party, you might
-
remember the taste
of the food,
-
the sight of a
particular lady,
-
the smell of her perfume,
-
topics, and so forth.
-
And this dinner
party shares
-
some overlapping features
with another event,
-
which was going dancing,
-
where he saw
the same lady,
-
smelled the same perfume,
-
although this time in
-
a dance hall with
-
many other things
happening.
-
And so James suggested
-
two things that are
really important here,
-
which are still part of
-
our understanding of how
memory is organized,
-
that memories
are built up of
-
associations
among elemental
-
components that come
-
together to form
the whole memory,
-
and the ways in
which we relate
-
one complex memory to
-
another comes
from pathways
-
from associations
that go from
-
the common elements of
-
one event to
another event.
-
At around the same
time, in Spain,
-
Santiago Ramon
y Cajal asked,
-
what is the structure
of individual neurons?
-
And here we begin
to get down
-
into the molecular
mechanisms,
-
the cellular mechanisms,
-
in this case,
of the brain.
-
He utilized a cell
staining method
-
from Camilio Golgi.
-
And from his work, from
-
his analysis of these
cell stainings,
-
he developed what he
-
called the
neuron doctrine,
-
and he argued that
the functional unit
-
of the brain
is the neuron.
-
And this was later
supported by
-
electrical
stimulation studies
-
of Luigi Galvani,
-
who stimulated with
electrical current
-
and showed that
you could activate
-
muscles and
other movement.
-
In 1906, Cajal and
-
Golgi shared the
Nobel Prize.
-
And here, to see what an
-
exquisite artist and
scientist he was,
-
you can see here the
-
rodent hippocampus
from 1911,
-
and it's just a
phenomenal level
-
of detail that he
-
was basically able to
both see and to share.
-
Charles Sherrington,
by the early 1900s,
-
asked, what is
the function
-
of neurons and synapses?
-
He published an
influential book called
-
The Integrative Action
of the Nervous System.
-
He coined the
term synapse,
-
which we use today
for the point of
-
contact between
two neurons,
-
and he studied
the behavior
-
of neurons that
were either
-
activated or inhibited
at the synapse and
-
he argued that
it's the balance
-
between activation
and inhibition,
-
which is key to
muscle movement.
-
And for this
accomplishment,
-
he won the 1932
Nobel Prize.
-
Moving across
back to European,
-
Ivan Pavlov in
Russia was asking,
-
how and where are
learned reflexes formed,
-
these sorts of
reflexes that
-
Descartes had talked
-
about many years
beforehand?
-
Now, this wasn't his
initial interest.
-
He initially was
interested in the
-
digestive glands of dogs.
-
But inadvertently,
he discovered how
-
reflexes, such
as salivation,
-
can be conditioned
or trained as
-
a learned response to
-
a previously neutral
cue, such as a bell.
-
And in exquisite
experiments
-
over the coming years,
-
he was able to
characterize
-
all the various parameters
-
by which learning
and extinction,
-
the unlearning
can take place.
-
And for this in 1904,
-
he was awarded
the Nobel Prize.
-
We come now to the last of
-
the historical
landmarks that
-
I wanted to
share with you.
-
We're up now into the
1940s and the 1950s,
-
and Alan Hodgkin and
Andrew Huxley asked,
-
how do neurons use
-
electrical signals
to communicate?
-
They developed what we
-
call the
Hodgkin-Huxley model,
-
which describes how the
firing in a neuron is
-
initiated and propagated
to the next neuron.
-
And from this,
-
they develop
biophysical models
-
of the electrical
characteristics of
-
neurons shown in the graph
-
there in the lower left.
-
And for this, they won
the 1963 Nobel Prize.
-
So over the last
15 minutes,
-
I've taken you
on a tour of
-
about 2000 years from
400 BC in Greece,
-
through the Roman Empire,
-
to the Muslim Middle
East and Iberia, France,
-
Europe, the US,
Spain, England,
-
Russia, and back
to the USA.
-
And we've seen
the development
-
of all of these various
questions that were
-
being answered or
addressed with
-
whatever technology was
-
available during the day.
-
So I said at
-
the beginning that we
-
can think about
the history
-
of neuroscience as being
-
three different
approaches:
-
approaches of philosophy,
-
of functional anatomy,
-
and cellular mechanisms.
-
Today, there's not
really much philosophy,
-
although there are
people who study
-
the philosophy of mind,
-
but the focus
of neuroscience
-
is on three
other areas now.
-
There is the
functional anatomy,
-
still, which is a
really important area.
-
There's cellular
mechanisms,
-
which we began to see
in some of the history.
-
But more recently,
-
there's been an
understanding of
-
molecular mechanisms,
-
what happens inside
the cells from
-
the molecules
that are moving
-
around inside and
between cells.
-
You'll see more of
this in the weeks to
-
come and the months to
come in this course.
-
Functional anatomy is now
-
a critical part of
behavioral neuroscience,
-
cognitive neuroscience,
and neuropsychology,
-
all of which will
be presented
-
in the weeks to come.
-
Cellular mechanisms
are critical part
-
of the quest for
neurophysiology,
-
for understanding
neuropharmacology,
-
and neuropsychology.
And some
-
of that you'll be
-
seeing in the next
few lectures.
-
Molecular mechanisms
have been
-
the most recent addition
-
to what we try to
understand about the brain.
-
It has drawn on the fields
-
of molecular
biology, genetics,
-
and protein chemistry
-
to really get us down to
-
the nuts and bolts of
-
how the brain works
at a molecular level,
-
which helps us
understand at
-
the cellular level
and which, in turn,
-
leads us to understand
-
the functional
organization of the brain.
-
Now, all of these
-
are basic science
endeavors,
-
and that's the main
focus of this course.
-
But all of this has
-
an impact on neurology
and psychiatry,
-
the understanding
of brain disorders
-
and mental health
disorders.
-
And as we understand
functional anatomy,
-
cellular mechanisms,
-
and molecular mechanisms
of the brain,
-
they help us
understand some of
-
the neurological and
psychiatric syndromes
-
that are presented
in medicine.
-
And so with this
-
as an overview of
the history of
-
neuroscience and how this
-
relates to the structure
-
of the current field
of neuroscience,
-
you're now ready to begin
-
what I hope is going to be
-
a very exciting course in
-
the semester
ahead that will
-
introduce you to the
field of neuroscience.