-
Professor Espino Perez:
Can you hear us, Rick?
-
Rick: Yeah, I can now.
-
I forgot I didn't have
on my headphones.
-
Professor Espino Perez: I
thought it might be helpful
-
to go over some of
the assignments
-
that are due next week.
-
Can everyone see
the Canvas page?
-
Rick: Yes.
-
Professor: Espino Perez: Great.
-
I opened up Module 4,
-
which will include
the introduction,
-
and I just wanted to give you
-
a brief overview of that today.
-
I have a video
-
I recorded for another
Research Methods class,
-
and I can put that up as well,
-
but we'll be talking
about it today.
-
That peer review is not
happening. Here we go.
-
There's an introduction
template that I wrote
-
that could help you
write the paper.
-
I also want you to check out
the actual submission link.
-
Within the submission link,
-
you'll get more information
-
about how to write
the introduction,
-
but I think the most
important component
-
of this will be to take
a look at the rubric
-
because when you take
a look at the rubric,
-
you'll be able to tell
-
what I'm placing most
of the attention on.
-
This whole thing is out of 50,
-
but in reality,
-
the introduction will
only be worth 25 points,
-
so you have to divide it by two.
-
As you can see,
-
just by looking at
the point values,
-
what really matters is
-
the main body of the Intro,
-
so the Literature Review.
-
Most of you are already
working on that
-
with your Assignment 1B,
-
looking for different
articles that might be
-
relevant to your
topic of interest,
-
but let's go through
the opening paragraph.
-
First, you have to put a title
-
before your first paragraph.
-
That's half a point.
-
Should I make it bigger,
or is it too small?
-
Rick: A little bigger
would be better.
-
Professor Espino Perez: I'll
do that. Give me a second.
-
How about there?
-
Is that good? Okay.
-
Title before the first paragraph
-
is half a point.
-
A catchy opening hook
-
to grab the reader's attention.
-
Hailey, what's your topic?
-
Hailey: It's about
sexism in the workplace
-
for women in higher
positions of power.
-
Professor Espino Perez: It could
be like a catchy hook.
-
It could be cheesy right
now. It doesn't matter.
-
This is going to be really bad,
-
but I just thought of it.
-
Imagine you're a woman at work
-
and one of your male colleagues
-
smacks your butt on your way in.
-
Though this is less likely
-
to be a reality for women now,
-
this is really common in
the '50s and the '60s.
-
Although overt sexual
harassment is less likely
-
to happen now to women
in the workplace,
-
sexual harassment is
still an issue for women,
-
and then you can keep talking.
-
If I read that, I'd
be like, my God, no.
-
I want to know what
happens to this guy
-
if he smacked my butt,
something like that.
-
State the big picture or
problem and its importance.
-
Although sexual
harassment might not be
-
as overt as it was before,
-
women in the workplace
still experience
-
sexual harassment,
-
so that is the importance
in the big picture.
-
Frames the context of the
story to be discussed.
-
It depends on what exactly
you'll be looking at.
-
Hailey, what exactly
will you be looking at,
-
so like perceptions
of sexual harassment
-
or the behaviors?
-
Hailey: I really want to look at
-
how it affects the
women and their ability
-
to continue on in
their career choice.
-
Professor Espino Perez: Got it, so
maybe you'll recruit women,
-
and you'll have
them recall a time
-
when they've experienced
sexual harassment
-
or imagine a scenario
where they experienced
-
sexual harassment
-
and then assess the likelihood
-
that they'd be able to
continue in their job,
-
that they'd want to persist,
-
that they feel
like they're safe,
-
that they could seek mentors,
-
and so on and so forth.
-
When you're framing the context
-
of the story to be discussed,
-
you're going to talk about
what your interest is,
-
and then you'll provide
a brief outline
-
of the paper.
-
"In this research paper,
-
I will be discussing
historical articles
-
on women's experiences of
-
sexual harassment
in the workplace,
-
the types of sexual harassment
-
that are typically
likely to occur,
-
other people's perceptions of
-
sexual harassment
in the workplace,
-
and finally, women's
likelihood of
-
persisting in the
workplace after
-
experiencing a trauma,
-
like sexual harassment,
in the workplace."
-
That summary is
basically going to be
-
the four or five articles
-
you're going to talk about.
-
You're just talking
about the main points,
-
and then boom,
-
you're done with the
opening paragraph.
-
The problems are things
-
that you might get
dock points on.
-
I would try to just get
as many points here.
-
I won't really dock points here,
-
but I will note
these as problems
-
so that you can work
on it in the future.
-
If Introduction appears
-
instead of the paper's title,
-
no, I want your title.
-
If you don't introduce properly
-
before getting into details,
-
so you don't have a
catchy opening hook,
-
that is not great.
-
The statement of the problem
-
is too brief or lacking.
-
It does not provide a
framework for the story,
-
so you're not telling me
-
what you will tell
me in the future
-
or how you're going to outline
-
or make an argument
-
for your research paper
and other things.
-
It could be like citing
issues and things like that.
-
Then for the Literature Review,
-
this is going to be the
bulk of your paper.
-
These will be the
four or five studies
-
that you'll be talking about
-
that you may have already done
-
for your Assignment 1B.
-
I want you to describe
-
the relevant parts
of each study.
-
Hailey, have you read any papers
-
for your study yet?
-
Hailey: I haven't completed
the assignment yet,
-
but I have five articles
picked out for it.
-
Professor Espino Perez: Cool.
-
Hailey: One is how
male allies can help
-
women in the workplace,
-
so that one just
gives an account
-
from male allies
in the workplace
-
and how they have
helped in the past
-
and how the women
want them to help
-
in the future.
-
Professor Espino Perez: I
think that would be great.
-
It might not be one of
the first paragraphs.
-
It might be one of the
ending paragraphs,
-
but I would want you to evaluate
-
the importance of each study
-
and describe the
relevant parts,
-
so how did they collect
data from these males,
-
how did they identify allies,
-
what behaviors did they describe
-
as allied behavior,
-
and how did women perceive
-
these behaviors?
-
That would be good.
I also want you to
-
tie findings together and
exploit common themes,
-
so across all of the five papers
-
that you're reading, of course,
-
they will be distinct,
-
but they will share
a similar theme
-
of women experiencing
sexual harassment
-
in the workplace.
-
I want you to find
similarities and differences
-
between them and also to cite
-
your articles in APA format.
-
This is why the citing
paper comes into play.
-
I'll be grading
that today. Great.
-
The problems would be
-
an insufficient
literature review,
-
so maybe you just talk
about two studies.
-
This will happen
before you give me
-
the title of the paper,
-
but you don't really tell me
-
what the paper was about.
-
That really doesn't
give me an idea
-
of what kind-of research you've read
-
that leads you to
ask this question.
-
I don't want all the details
-
about the study methods.
-
I don't want you to tell me,
-
"They recruited males
from Portland, Oregon,
-
on Tuesday and Thursday because
-
those are the days
that males go out."
-
I don't want to know
all that information.
-
I just want to know general.
-
You don't discuss the
importance of each study,
-
so, of course,
describe the study.
-
Main claims and supporting
-
evidence not clear
from citations.
-
Again, don't just put
the title of the study.
-
Give me some summary.
-
Summarizes article after article
-
instead of synthesizing
research findings.
-
I do want you to
include a summary,
-
but you should also have
-
a common thread throughout and
-
talk about how the articles
inform one another
-
and how they helped you
-
form your research question.
-
Improper APA
citations and other.
-
Of course, APA is going to
be important throughout.
-
After you write the bulk
-
of your research or
your literature review,
-
you'll have a paragraph
identifying gaps
-
to motivate your study.
-
For instance, for Hailey,
-
it would be something like,
-
"Although research has examined
-
how male allies can help women
-
experiencing sexual harassment
-
in the workplace,"
-
this might not be true,
but I'm just spitballing,
-
"limited research
has investigated
-
how sexual harassment affects
-
women's persistence
in the workplace
-
and what strategies
women can engage in
-
for themselves to deal
with sexual harassment
-
in the workplace."
-
You'd compare and contrast
findings across studies.
-
This is where you can
include a sentence or two
-
for each of the studies
-
that you've already discussed.
-
Talk about the limitations
of previous work
-
and gaps in the literature.
-
Sometimes allies are people
-
who exhibit sexual harassment
-
towards other people,
-
so maybe talk about how
allies are important,
-
but they can also
be perpetrators
-
of sexual harassment, etc.
-
Reiterate your main claims
and outline evidence
-
to set the context for
your current study.
-
Have APA citations throughout.
-
If you don't compare
and contrast,
-
that's not great,
-
if you don't critically evaluate
-
the limitations of the
work, that's not great,
-
if you don't summarize
everything that
-
you've talked about so far
-
before jumping into
this current study,
-
that's also not great.
-
Then your final paragraph
will be talking about
-
your study more specifically.
-
For Hailey, "The purpose
of my study is to see
-
how women experience
sexual harassment
-
in the workplace
-
and how that might
affect their persistence
-
in a mostly male-dominated
job," or something.
-
Describe how your
experimental variables
-
will be manipulated
and measured.
-
Participants will be assigned
to one of two conditions.
-
In Condition 1, participants
will be asked to
-
either recall or
imagine a scenario
-
where they experience
sexual harassment,
-
or in the second scenario,
-
participants will
be asked to recall
-
one of their job
experiences, just generally.
-
Then all participants
will complete
-
measures of persistence
in the workplace
-
and other things,
-
whatever your dependent
variables are.
-
Then your testable hypotheses.
-
"I predict that people in
-
the sexual harassment condition
-
will exhibit lower
levels of persistence
-
in the workplace
-
compared to women who are just
-
in the 'describe your
typical job scenario.'"
-
The problems might be
lacking theoretical support,
-
but that's where
your Intro comes in.
-
That's where you're
talking about
-
the articles that
actually helped you
-
come up with this theory.
-
I do want you to talk about
your IVs and your DVs,
-
so maybe you'll talk
about persistence.
-
Persistence will be measured by
-
the everyday persistence
scale created
-
by Chatterley and
colleagues in 2020.
-
If you don't tell me
what your hypothesis is,
-
then that's not great,
-
if I don't know what
your hypothesis is
-
or it seems unclear because
-
of the lack of information
from prior studies,
-
it's also not great,
-
and if it's unclear how
your study will test
-
the hypothesis, it's
also not great.
-
Let's say Hailey
said something like,
-
"I'm interested in
seeing persistence rates
-
among women who experienced
sexual harassment,
-
so I'm going to recruit
10 male participants,
-
and I'm going to ask them
about how many times
-
they have sexually
harassed a woman."
-
That's not answering the
question that she posed.
-
That's completely different.
-
Now your References.
-
Again, references are
pretty important,
-
APA style,
-
so having a title that
says "References"
-
using APA style,
-
everything you
cited in your paper
-
appears in the References,
-
and having at least four
papers that you can cite.
-
Of course, you should
already have five
-
with Assignment 1B, so
that should be great.
-
The problems could be,
-
it's not formatted correctly,
-
APA style isn't followed
or used inconsistently,
-
so that's why I'm going to try
-
to get you feedback
by the end of today,
-
if not the end of tomorrow,
-
on your [inaudible]
-
so that you know, if
you did anything wrong,
-
what you can do to improve.
-
They have to be in
alphabetical order.
-
If you use less
than four sources,
-
you will be docked points.
-
It does not match the text.
-
Hailey, if you cited
Chatterley and colleagues
-
in 2020,
-
but then you use Tingle
and colleagues 2020,
-
then I would be like,
-
what happened to the
other citation?
-
Too many secondary
sources cited.
-
This would be like
using data from CNN
-
to see how many women experience
-
sexual harassment
-
and not enough
empirical articles.
-
You can use secondary sources,
-
but try not to use
more than two.
-
Then just overall format,
-
your organization, your
grammar, your spelling,
-
the length is sufficient for
-
the full picture of the problem,
-
your margins are good,
-
your double spacing is good,
-
your indentation's good,
-
you're using 12-point font,
-
and you have page numbers.
-
You don't start out
broadly and end narrowly.
-
Of course, Hailey,
with your paper,
-
you're going to be, like,
-
"Women used to experience
sexual harassment."
-
That'll be really broad,
-
and then you'll go
very detailed because
-
you'll talk about your
experiment specifically.
-
If you use quotes rather
than paraphrasing,
-
it is a lot easier
to paraphrase.
-
If you're having trouble
paraphrasing something,
-
let me know, and I can help.
-
Using active rather
than passive voice.
-
Don't give personal opinions.
-
If your margins
are inconsistent,
-
your double spacing
is inconsistent,
-
consistent font is not used,
-
page number is inconsistent
and in the wrong place,
-
your Spell Check isn't used,
-
there are choppy transitions,
-
there are fragmented sentences,
-
the length is insufficient
-
to capture the
breath of a problem.
-
After all that, you
can get 50 points,
-
you divide it by 20,
-
and that will be
worth 25 points.
-
Just to let you know,
-
I do have a way of grading
all of these papers.
-
The first draft is worth less,
-
and then the second
draft is worth more,
-
then the final draft
is worth the most.
-
I do that because I know
you're still learning
-
how to write a paper,
-
so if you do get docked
points on the Intro,
-
I want you to be able
to gain those points
-
by doing better on
the second draft.
-
The points that you
might lose here
-
aren't as meaningful
as they would be
-
for the second or
the third draft.
-
Once you write the second draft,
-
it will include a revised Intro,
-
and then your first draft
of your Method and Results.
-
Then your final draft will
include your revised Intro,
-
revised Method and Results,
and a Discussion section.
-
You're always going
to get feedback,
-
and my goal is
-
not for you to get it
perfect the first time
-
because I remember this
was really hard for me
-
when I learned how
to write a paper,
-
so I want to see improvement.
-
I don't expect everyone
to get a perfect score,
-
but I would like to
see improvement.
-
Are there any questions here?
-
I will be uploading a
sample paper as well,
-
but I did want to just give
you a brief introduction.
-
How do I write an introduction?
-
I feel the rubric is
good for telling you,
-
these are the things that you
-
should and shouldn't
do to get the points,
-
but I wrote this template
that you can use
-
when you're writing
your introduction.
-
Let me open it up
in Word document
-
because this looks weird,
-
so I'm going to stop the share,
-
and let me get my
Word document open.
-
That's a lot of information
-
I've thrown at you-all.
-
Any questions so far?
-
I really want to read
Hailey's paper now.
-
Let me share the
screen again. Great.
-
I'm going to make it bigger.
-
I created this document
-
to help you on the
upcoming assignment.
-
You can read all of this,
-
but the author of your
textbook recommends
-
you write the main
points of an argument
-
and individual research findings
-
on a separate index card,
-
so then you can rearrange them
-
to figure out what
makes the most sense.
-
Let me get the sun
out of my face.
-
You can also do this by
writing the topic sentence
-
of each paragraph in
this Word document
-
and then finding support
for your arguments
-
from the five or more
articles you've reviewed
-
for Assignment 1B.
-
Things to keep in mind.
-
Your Intro should be between
-
five to maybe seven paragraphs.
-
That might be anywhere
between 3-5 pages,
-
so it shouldn't be that intense.
-
The most difficult
part is finding
-
how to weave the
entire narrative
-
through the article
summaries that you have.
-
The first sentence
of each paragraph
-
should be a topic sentence.
-
For instance, if Hailey is
-
going to be talking
about male allies,
-
so she could say something like,
-
"Men, not only women,
can provide support
-
for those who are experiencing
sexual harassment
-
and also to prevent
sexual harassment
-
from re-occurring,"
-
and then you'll go
into that study.
-
The topic sentence makes a claim
-
that the following
sentences support
-
using research
findings as evidence.
-
Is it Chatterley?
-
Chatterley and colleagues
conducted a study
-
where men were recruited,
and so on and so forth,
-
and then you go on to the
Summary of the study.
-
Respected and
experienced researchers
-
do not use many, if any, quotes
-
when writing, so
try to paraphrase.
-
You can always paraphrase.
-
When discussing the
findings of past research,
-
only provide as
much detail about
-
the researcher's
method as is necessary
-
for your reader to
understand the results.
-
I don't want how many
people they collected.
-
I don't want when
they collected it.
-
I just want maybe to know
-
that all the participants
-
were self-identified as male and
-
what questions they were asked
-
about allied behaviors.
-
If I'm curious about the
methodological details,
-
I think it's really interesting,
-
I can always look up
the information myself
-
by using the references
that you've cited.
-
Rick: [inaudible]
-
Professor Espino Perez:
Sure. Go ahead.
-
Rick: This is all
for the Intro.
-
Professor Espino Perez: Yeah. Do
you have a question about it,
-
or is it [inaudible] ?
-
Rick: No. You just
caught me right there.
-
I just go, okay, that's a lot,
-
but I'm following it.
-
Professor Espino Perez: It is a lot.
-
I feel like I would
rather give you
-
more information
than not enough.
-
Rick: This is helpful. A
guideline is what I need.
-
Professor Espino Perez: Yeah.
This is two pages long,
-
so I'll show you what I created.
-
This is just like things
-
to keep in mind generally,
-
and then I have
paragraph outlines
-
that you can use,
-
and they can end each paragraph
-
with a sentence communicating
the implications
-
of a finding you discussed
in the paragraph.
-
I need you to answer
the question,
-
so what about the
research you summarized?
-
This tells the reader
-
what they should have taken away
-
from that paragraph.
-
"The research from
Chatterley and colleagues
-
suggests that males
are important sources
-
of preventing future instances
-
of sexual harassment."
-
These are outlines
for your paper,
-
so you could use this.
-
Begin your Introduction
with a hook,
-
something that is general.
-
What did I say?
-
"Imagine you walk into work,
-
and your male colleague
slaps your behind."
-
Though this might not represent
-
the sexual harassment
-
that women experience
in the workplace,
-
women do still experience
sexual harassment.
-
The present study
will explore the ways
-
in which women experience
sexual harassment
-
and the consequences
of sexual harassment
-
for persistence
in the workplace.
-
First, I will outline,
-
next, we will discuss,
-
and then following, blah-blah,
-
and then finally,
-
you talk about your
actual experiment.
-
First, I said, okay;
-
so then your first
paragraph follows.
-
That could be the first
paragraph in your Intro.
-
Does that make sense?
-
It's something really broad.
-
You talk about your
present study,
-
and then you give an outline
-
of what you'll be
talking about,
-
so you're telling me
-
what main points I should get.
-
The next sentence
should introduce
-
how your hook relates to
your research question.
-
Though this might not represent
-
the sexual harassment
-
that women experience
in the workplace today,
-
women do still experience
sexual harassment.
-
The next few sentences
should discuss
-
existing research
broadly and also
-
outline the limits of
the current research,
-
how your research question
-
will address the existing gap,
-
so these sentences
should foreshadow
-
what you will be discussing
-
in the following paragraphs.
-
The present study
will explore the ways
-
in which women experience
sexual harassment and
-
the consequences of
sexual harassment
-
for persistence in
the workplace,
-
so first, you're going
to outline blah.
-
Next, we'll discuss that,
-
following blah blah
blah, and then finally,
-
I will explore how these
studies tie together,
-
but I'd be more specific
-
to inform the current study.
-
The final sentence should
be about your study
-
that outline your IV
and your levels of it,
-
your dependent variable
and your predictions,
-
and so on and so forth.
-
Rick: I have a question.
-
Professor Espino Perez:
Yes, go for it.
-
Rick: About the existing
gap, I didn't follow.
-
Could you give an example
-
or explain a little bit?
-
I missed really.
-
Professor Espino Perez: Sure.
-
Rick: How your research
question will address that.
-
Professor Espino Perez:
Limited research to date
-
has directly examined
the consequences
-
of sexual harassment
-
for persistence
in the workplace.
-
Rick: You want to show work
-
that needs to be
done but hasn't.
-
Professor Espino Perez:
You can say that
-
about mostly any study because
-
there are specific studies
that are out there,
-
but there are always ways
to make studies unique.
-
Rick: Right there at that point,
-
you want to state how yours
is going to be unique,
-
how this study is
going to fill that?
-
Professor Espino Perez: Yeah.
-
Rick, your study is
about COVID-19, right?
-
Rick: Yeah, depression due
to isolation from COVID.
-
Professor Espino Perez: Right,
so your Intro could be
-
something like,
-
in 2019 and before,
-
most people would say that
-
they'd love to stay home and
-
watch Netflix all day.
-
In 2020, that became a reality,
-
but what people didn't
anticipate is that
-
they would experience
-
increasing levels of loneliness.
-
The COVID-19 pandemic
was unprecedented,
-
and people may have experienced
-
increased sense of
loneliness and depression.
-
Almost no research to
date exists to examine
-
how pandemics and
quarantining has effects
-
on people's emotional
and psychological state.
-
The current research
will explore blah
-
because that's, like,
brand-new research.
-
Rick: I did find some good ones.
-
Professor Espino Perez:
There have been papers
-
that have been published
really quickly,
-
but prior to those papers,
-
there's been really
nothing because
-
we weren't doing this research
-
during the Spanish flu,
-
but those statements can be made
-
almost about any paper,
-
thus, the present
study will explore
-
how these studies tie together
-
to inform the current study.
-
This would be a lot better.
-
This is just like
me spitballing.
-
Also, you should know,
writing is hard.
-
This is not something
I would submit,
-
but this would be
something that I'm like,
-
okay, that's a good start,
-
and then I would
go back and edit.
-
I wouldn't anticipate
writing this paper
-
the day before and
submitting it.
-
I would, maybe, set a
timer for 30 minutes
-
and say, I'm just going
to outline this paper.
-
No matter how crap it is,
-
it's still better than nothing,
-
and you can always work with it
-
to make it better.
-
That's the Intro paragraph.
-
Then I want you to
support your argument.
-
Write the support for
your argument statement
-
as the beginning
of the paragraph.
-
Based on the literature
review you did,
-
what articles or findings
support your argument?
-
Write a few sentences based
on these articles here,
-
reiterate your point,
and create a bridge
-
between this paragraph
and the next.
-
Repeat until your argument
is clearly fleshed out.
-
Not to put you on
the spot, Hailey,
-
but I am putting
you on the spot.
-
Give me some titles
of the papers
-
that you've read.
-
Hailey: Give me just one second.
-
One is "A Confirmatory
Study of the Relations
-
Between Workplace Sexism,
Sense of Belonging,
-
Mental Health, and
Job Satisfaction
-
Among Women in
Male-Dominated Industries."
-
Professor Espino Perez:
Give me a sec.
-
Workplace harassment,
belonging, and what else?
-
Hailey: Mental health
and job satisfaction.
-
Professor Espino Perez:
What's another one?
-
Hold on.
-
Hailey: Another one is "Harmful
Workplace Experiences
-
and Women’s Occupational
Wellbeing:
-
A Meta-Analysis."
-
Professor Espino Perez: You know
what a meta-analysis is?
-
Hailey: From what I've read,
-
isn't it just like the
author of that article
-
is taking findings
from other studies
-
and compiling it into one,
-
so it's not his
personal research.
-
It's him comparing a
bunch of other research.
-
Professor Espino Perez: Yeah,
which might be helpful
-
because you'll be able to read,
-
like, 20 research articles
in that one article,
-
so yes, that's good. Any other?
-
Hailey: "Women and Women
of Color and Leadership:
-
Complexity, Identity,
and Intersectionality."
-
Professor Espino Perez: Let's
just go with these three.
-
You could say something,
one of your paragraphs
-
following the Intro could be;
-
"women who experience sexual
assessment, may have..."
-
okay, or, "Sexual harassment may
lead to decreased levels
-
of belonging, job satisfaction,
-
and occupational well-being,"
-
so then I'd talk about
these two articles.
-
This could be two paragraphs.
-
These two articles, I
would talk about here.
-
Maybe what I would do is
-
I would make that
a subheading and
-
make that a subheading,
-
and so that will bolster
your argument there.
-
"This may be particularly true
-
for women in
male-dominated fields."
-
Maybe that could be
the second paragraph.
-
Does that make sense? Yeah.
-
Hailey: Yeah.
-
Professor Espino Perez:
The first sentence
-
of the second paragraph
-
might be something more
specific than that,
-
but that could be
your first sentence
-
for the first paragraph,
-
something like this for
the second paragraph,
-
and these are the articles
that you will use
-
to support those statements,
-
and so on and so forth.
-
Then the third paragraph
could be something like,
-
"Women with multiple
stigmatized identities
-
may experience the
negative effects...
-
negative effects of workplace harassment."
-
Saying "multiple" may compound
-
the negative effects of
workplace harassment
-
for women or something.
-
Then you'll talk about this,
-
"Women and Women of
Color in Leadership."
-
Then your fourth paragraph might
-
be something about allies
-
and preventing future
sexual harassment,
-
and then your final paragraph.
-
There you go.
-
I outlined your paper, Hailey.
-
It doesn't have to be that.
-
You can rearrange it.
-
Does that make sense, Alessia?
-
I can do this for
anyone else's paper.
-
I'm just picking on Hailey.
-
If you want me to help,
-
I can do this for
your paper too.
-
Then your final paragraph,
-
you'll summarize the arguments,
-
points listed in
Paragraphs 2 and 5.
-
"As stated earlier,
-
sexual harassment has
negative implications
-
for women.
-
Women who experience
sexual harassment
-
experience lower
levels of belonging
-
and job satisfaction, citation,
-
lower levels of occupational
well-being, citation.
-
Further, those who possess
-
multiple stigmatized identities,
-
so on and so forth, and then,
-
allies may help prevent
future sexual harassment,
-
and so on and so forth.
-
However, limited
work has examined
-
how sexual harassment can affect
-
not only job satisfaction
but also persistence.
-
In the current study,
-
I will examine how
sexual harassment
-
affects persistence among
college-aged women,
-
women of different ethnicities,"
whatever you want.
-
You can talk about your sample.
-
"I will manipulate
sexual harassment
-
by blah, blah, blah.
-
I will measure persistence
-
by using the Everyday
Workplace,"
-
this doesn't exist by the way,
-
"Persistence Scale,
Chatterley and Tingle,
-
2020, which includes items like,
-
'I want to burn my
office down,' on
-
a scale from, 1,
strongly disagree
-
to 7, strongly agree,"
-
just to give me a sense of
-
what the scale measures.
-
"I often daydream about
burning my office down."
-
Then that's the end.
That's your Intro.
-
Rick: [inaudible]
-
Professor Espino Perez:
Does that make sense?
-
Alicia, does that make sense?
-
Rick: I'm looking at it, but I
-
just want to say it out loud.
-
On the final sentence,
-
you're going to bring in
-
your references
and say what they
-
offered like a summary thing.
-
Professor Espino Perez: Then
finally, you're going to say,
-
"I predict that women
who are in Condition 1
-
will want to burn
their office down
-
more than those who
are in Condition 2,"
-
even though that's not
-
what you were
originally measuring.
-
I messed up. That
should be persistence,
-
I'm not burning
your office down,
-
but you get what
I'm saying, right?
-
Rick: The last part, this is
the direction I am going.
-
Here's the questions
that haven't
-
been asked or answered
or something.
-
Professor Espino Perez: Yeah, so
Hailey, I can send you this.
-
I'm not sure how
helpful it'll be,
-
but anyone else who
comes to these meetings,
-
I'm more than happy
to use your study.
-
Perfect.
-
Does that make it less
intimidating, I guess?
-
I created this
just for your sake
-
and also for my sake because
-
I want better papers.
-
Then your support
for your argument,
-
so this will be paragraphs 2-5,
-
and then your final
paragraph, paragraph 6.
-
The end. That's it.
-
Also, remember I do
offer extra credit
-
for visiting the Writing Center,
-
and you can get extra credit
-
for every time that
you submit a paper,
-
so for the Intro, for
the Method and Results,
-
and for the Discussion.
-
Schedule an appointment today
-
if you want to get
that extra credit,
-
and they'll also help
you write your paper.
-
Rick: On that should we state,
-
is it the timing of
when our thing is
-
before the paper,
-
or should we state in our thing
-
with the Writing Center
-
what we want to go
over them about?
-
Professor Espino Perez: Do you mean,
-
should you schedule
your appointment
-
before the assignment is due?
-
Rick: No, just to get credit
-
for what we're going over.
-
It's just the timing of
the the tutor assigned.
-
Professor Espino Perez: You
can go over anything.
-
You can go over APA formatting.
-
You can go over making
your logical argument.
-
You can go over
anything you want.
-
I just want you to visit
the Writing Center
-
and make use of the
resources we have available.
-
Rick: I wasn't able to
get in touch with Emily
-
for some reason.
-
Was that who you
were about to say
-
we also have?
-
Professor Espino Perez: Yes,
Emily. I'll reach out to her.
-
Could you email her?
-
Rick: Do you have
that information?
-
Professor Espino Perez: I
will get that information
-
and send it out to
everyone because
-
that's a tutor that we have
-
that's separate from
the Writing Center,
-
and she's experienced in 301,
-
so she can help with that.
-
Rick: I need to talk with her.
-
It's getting deep
on the stuff here,
-
so just to go over stuff,
-
and I'll feel more
comfortable in it.
-
A few things. Appreciate
that. Thank you.
-
Professor Espino Perez:
Of course, yeah.
-
I'll send it out to the group.
-
I wrote it down.
-
Alicia, how are you feeling?
-
Rick: I'm feeling okay.
-
Alicia: I've been having
a little bit of trouble
-
with finding studies.
-
I think it's because I'm not
-
sure, specifically,
keywords to use
-
or what to actually look for.
-
Professor Espino Perez:
Has anyone else
-
been having trouble with that?
-
Hailey, it seems like
you've got it all down.
-
Rick?
-
Rick: My keywords,
-
I think I had them
right off the bat.
-
Depression due to
COVID isolation.
-
It got me some real
pinpoint stuff.
-
They're using different methods.
-
It's nice. There's
one self-survey,
-
and then there's statistical and
-
physiological also.
-
Professor Espino Perez:
It's all great.
-
: Alicia, you scheduled an
appointment with me, right?
-
: I have not.
-
: Okay. Do you have
time after class?
-
: We might not go the full
hour so I can help you.
-
: Yeah.
-
: Okay. Anyone else?
If you want to help,
-
: schedule an appointment
or stay after class,
-
: and I will go
over those things.
-
: I do want to
finish the lecture
-
: today. Everyone
take a deep breath.
-
: The Intro will be fine.
-
: I need to take a deep breath
too because I feel like
-
: I talk so much that
I forget to breathe.
-
: The Introwill be fine.
-
: I'm more than
happy to meet with
-
: you during my office hours.
-
: I still have office
hours this week.
-
: I have advising hours
that no one is coming to.
-
: You can come to those
advising hours too.
-
: I can make time
to meet with you.
-
: I'm more than
happy to help you.
-
: I love research especially
when it's not mine.
-
: When it's my research,
I'm just like,
-
: "I don't want to do it,' but
I love helping other people.
-
: Let me know. Alright, so
let me get my PowerPoint.
-
: We're almost done
with the PowerPoint.
-
Rick: Where were we?
-
: Share screen.
-
Rick: Yes. I believe yes,
that is what I want.
-
Because I just got
my external monitor
-
back. Thank goodness.
-
We're going to continue
with Chapter five.
-
If you have a question,
-
just holla because I can't see
-
your faces now for some reason.
-
Identifying good measurement.
-
I do want to see
your faces, though.
-
What do I do? I'm
going to put here.
-
I can't see. Oh, man. I
can't see your faces.
-
Just speak out if you know
-
the answers to the
questions I'm going to ask.
-
Can you see the screen
-
without the notes or
can you see the notes?
-
Speaker 4: I just see you.
-
: Yeah, right now, it's basics.
-
: I see the school.
-
Speaker 3: Oh, my bad. Oh, man.
-
Let's see.
-
Speaker 4: There they are.
-
: Now.
-
: Perfect.
-
: Now I can see you.
-
: We talked about three
types of reliability.
-
: What are they?
-
: I didn't ask
questions last time,
-
: but I'll do it this time.
-
: The three types.
-
: Alicia. Anyone else?
-
: I don't know where
I'm at. Oh, my God.
-
: Yes, Hailey.
-
: Test reliability, interrater
-
: reliability, and
internal reliability.
-
Speaker 3: Perfect.
-
I won't make you
answer what they are.
-
But Rick or Alicia
or anyone else,
-
do you remember what
those mean? Test, retest.
-
Speaker 4: When you test or do
-
, I want to call it a study.
-
I don't know if that's the
right word, but basically,
-
do it at one point.
-
Speaker 3: We're looking at
-
test-retest reliability for IQ.
-
Speaker 4: For IQ? Say,
-
today you test someone's IQ
-
and they get a specific answer,
-
and then say next
week you test them
-
again and compare the results.
-
Speaker 3: Good test for test
reliability would tell us
-
what about the IQ scores?
-
Speaker 4: That it's an
accurate representation of
-
their IQ and it's
reliable data to use.
-
Speaker 3: Close.
Rick or Hailey?
-
Speaker 4: I think Meghan
raised their hand.
-
Speaker 3: Oh,
Meghan, go for it.
-
I didn't see your hand.
-
Speaker 4: It would
be consistent,
-
so it would be around
the same quantitatively?
-
Speaker 3: Yes, perfect.
Let's say at Time 1,
-
you get an IQ score
of 125 and Time 2,
-
you get an IQ score of 128,
that's relatively consistent.
-
I think that's what you meant
-
Alicia when you said accurate,
-
but it's about consistency,
-
so having almost the same score.
-
I would have low
test-retest reliability if
-
one day I score a
40 on an IQ test,
-
and then the next
time I score 140,
-
so that doesn't have good
test-retest reliability.
-
Perfect. We talked about
interrater reliability.
-
Does anyone want to
take a stab at that?
-
Maybe Meghan? Or Rick.
-
: Well, do the same test,
-
: but it's not you
doing it again.
-
: It's two different people
-
: doing the same
test on the same.
-
: That's what it is. You're
looking for consistent scores,
-
: but using two different
people doing this film.
-
Speaker 3: Yes, can you
give us an example?
-
I came up with really
silly examples,
-
so just go for it.
-
: I test to see if,
-
: I run around the
block and I get sweaty,
-
: and then someone else
-
: test to see if running
around the block.
-
: No, that's wrong. I
don't know. I can't think.
-
Speaker 3: No,
that's okay. I think
-
the key here is that
there are two observers.
-
Speaker 4: There's
two observers.
-
: Yes.
-
: But for the same testing.
-
Speaker 3: Yes. In your example,
-
Alicia would be my
research assistant,
-
and she would be coding for how
-
sweaty you get
versus someone else.
-
I would also have Hailey.
-
Interrater reliability
means that Alicia
-
and Hailey score you similarly
on how sweaty you get,
-
and Hailey and Alicia score
-
the other person similarly
in how sweaty they get.
-
It's two observers looking at
-
behavior in someone else.
Does that make sense?
-
Speaker 4: Yeah, thank you.
-
Speaker 3: Yeah, I think
you were almost there.
-
You just had the
two people down,
-
but not sure where it
went, but that's good.
-
That's still good. What
about internal reliability?
-
Speaker 4: Isn't this where
if R is positive and strong,
-
you have a good reliability?
-
Speaker 3: Yes, but
what does that mean?
-
Speaker 4: I'm not
quite positive.
-
Speaker 3: Who was that?
-
Speaker 4: Grace.
-
Speaker 3: Grace. Yeah.
Good job, Grace. Hailey.
-
Speaker 4: Isn't it where, when
-
you ask questions to the people,
-
so it's like two
different questions,
-
but they both are trying
to get at the same answer.
-
They're just worded differently,
-
and the people answer
them consistently.
-
: Yes. Rick.
-
: Same question,
word it differently.
-
Speaker 3: Yes. In Hailey's
questionnaire that I created,
-
it would be if you ask people,
-
how likely are you or how
-
often do you daydream about
burning your office down?
-
Then also, if your
office was burning down,
-
would you call the
fire department?
-
Those are questions that
are framed differently,
-
but also measuring the same,
-
like, how much do you
-
appreciate or want
to work at your job?
-
: Oh, okay.
-
: Yeah. They would
answer consistently.
-
: Good. I'm going to keep
-
: asking these questions
just to make sure.
-
: It's okay if you don't
get the entire measure
-
: or the type of
reliability, 100% there.
-
: As you can tell, most of you
were 80% there. It's okay.
-
: And during the exam,
it will be open book,
-
: open note at the
end of the term,
-
: which is also, I guess,
the end of the year.
-
: Still good job. We
talked about this.
-
: We looked at the measurement
of head circumference,
-
: at Time 1 and Time 2.
-
: Which reliability
would this be?
-
: You measure the
head at Time 1,
-
: you measure the
head at Time 2,
-
: and the measures are pretty
related to one another.
-
: Which of the reliabilities
would this be?
-
Speaker 4: Test-retest.
-
Speaker 3: Yeah. Perfect.
-
This is interrater reliability.
-
Does anyone want to
explain the difference
-
between the left and
the right to me?
-
Speaker 4: The left one is
showing that both observers,
-
their ratings are very
close to one another,
-
whereas the right
side is showing
-
they're all over the place and
-
not quite matching
up with one another.
-
Speaker 3: Perfect. You
would say that this one has
-
high interrater reliability and
-
this one has low
interrater reliability
-
because we have two observers,
-
so Mark's ratings
and Matt's ratings,
-
and they both seem to
track on the same line.
-
But if we have Mark's
ratings and Peter's ratings,
-
they don't seem to track on
-
the same line;
they're scattered.
-
This would mean that this has
low interrater reliability,
-
so I might not trust
Peter's ratings.
-
Maybe he's using a
different assessment
-
for sweatiness if we're testing
-
Rick and other people's
sweatiness when they run,
-
but Mark and Matt seem to
-
be on point with what
they're measuring.
-
I believe this is where we were,
-
and this is what Grace
was talking about.
-
Using the correlation
coefficient
-
r to evaluate reliability.
-
The correlation
coefficient finds
-
the relationship between
two different variables.
-
Someone give me two
variables that could be
-
related or maybe not related.
-
: Foot size and height.
-
: Foot size and height.
We have foot size
-
: being X and height being Y.
-
: We could use all
of these different
-
: scatter plots to
represent that.
-
: Another thing to note for
the correlation coefficient,
-
: the values go from
negative one to one,
-
: so it includes zero.
-
: The sign in front
of the number,
-
: whether it's
negative or positive,
-
: tells you the slope direction.
-
: If the slope seems to be
increasing like this one,
-
: all the data seem
to be increasing,
-
: as X increases, Y increases;
-
: you would say that this
is a positive slope.
-
: As X increases as shoe size
increases, height increases.
-
: This would also be
a positive slope.
-
: This one, on the other hand,
-
: would be a negative
slope because as
-
: shoe size increases,
height decreases.
-
: If you're a size, let's
just say this is 11,
-
: then you are five foot or
four foot two or something.
-
: as shoe size increases,
height decreases.
-
: But this one doesn't really
have a slope because there's
-
: no way to predict as X
increases what Y would be,
-
: since it's all
scattered around.
-
: You can see what the
slope direction is or
-
: whether there's a positive
value or negative value
-
: by looking at the slope
of the pattern of the dots.
-
: Does that make
sense? Then you can
-
: tell the strength by how
closely together the dots are.
-
: I would imagine a line going
straight diagonally here,
-
: and how well do the dots
match up to that line?
-
: They don't match
up super well here.
-
: This is a 0.56 relationship.
-
: Again, from negative
one to one, this one,
-
: if you had a line,
-
: they all seem to
track really well.
-
: This is an r of 0.93.
-
: You can tell that it's
really strong by how well
-
: the dots adhere
to one another.
-
: This one is -0.59,
-
: so it's a little better
than this one or a
-
: little stronger
than this example.
-
: This r of 0.01,
-
: there's no strength, it's
not really predictive.
-
: Perfect. Let me see
-
: my notes if I need to
tell you anything else.
-
: Any questions here?
-
: Which is the strongest
r in these scatter plots?
-
Speaker 4: R 0.93.
-
Speaker 3: Perfect.
If it was a -0.93,
-
would it still be the strongest?
-
Or actually, negative 0.94.
-
Speaker 4: I think so because
-
negative correlations
can also be strong.
-
It just depends on
what you're measuring.
-
Speaker 3: Perfect.
It doesn't really
-
matter whether it has a
positive or negative.
-
It's more about the absolute
value of the r. Of these,
-
the r 0.93 is the strongest,
-
and which one is the weakest,
-
Rick or Alicia, or Grace?
-
Speaker 4: I can't
see the numbers,
-
but the scattering ones.
-
Speaker 3: This one.
-
: That one. 0.01, yeah.
-
: Perfect. How does
the correlation
-
: coefficient
evaluate reliability
-
: for the test-retest
reliability?
-
: You would correlate
their first IQ score to
-
: their second IQ score
and see how strong it is.
-
: If your first IQ score was
-
: 125 and your second
IQ score was 126,
-
: they would correlate
pretty highly.
-
: Your r would be very positive.
-
: If r is strong and positive,
-
: at least 0.05 or above,
you have good test.
-
: I think it should
be 0.50 or above.
-
: Then you have good
test-retest reliability.
-
: If r is positive at weak,
-
: this is a sign of low
test-retest reliability.
-
: This reliability
is only relevant for
-
: variables that don't
change over time.
-
: It wouldn't be relevant
for something like weight.
-
: Weight tends to fluctuate
more than shoe size, maybe.
-
: What about for
interrater reliability?
-
: Two observers rate the same
-
: participants at the same time,
-
: and then r is computed.
-
: You want to see how Hailey and
-
: Alicia's evaluation of
sweating after running,
-
: how related they
are to one another.
-
: Here, if r is
positive and strong,
-
: the value has to
be 0.7 or higher;
-
: you have good
interrater reliability.
-
: Here, you want it to be
positive, not negative.
-
: If r is positive but weak,
-
: you don't have good
interrater reliability.
-
: Negative r in this situation
would be meaningful,
-
: and it would mean terrible
interrater reliability.
-
: If you had a -0.7,
-
: that would mean that when
Hailey rates the person
-
: as 5 on sweatiness,
-
: Alicia would rate them
as -5 on sweatiness.
-
: As Hailey's scores go up,
-
: Alicia's scores go down,
-
: or the other way around.
-
: Internal reliability, this is
-
: when you have
more than one item
-
: to tap into the construct.
-
: This is like the
example that I gave with
-
: Hailey's burning
the building down.
-
: I would correlate
the two values
-
: to see how related
they are to one another.
-
: Another important
thing to note is,
-
: I asked one question
where positive means more
-
: likely to burn the
employment place down.
-
: The other question, I said,
-
: how likely are you to
-
: prevent the burning
of the place down?
-
: I need to reverse
code that item,
-
: and we'll talk about
what that means,
-
: and then correlate
the items to
-
: see how well that
taps into the construct
-
: of workplace satisfaction.
Does that make sense?
-
: I must hear more about
reverse code. I like that.
-
Speaker 3: We'll
talk about that, and
-
I'll show you how to do that,
-
but maybe not in this lecture.
-
Essentially, what
you want to do with
-
reverse coding is you want to
-
make sure that all of your
items mean the same thing.
-
I want to tap into
the construct of
-
workplace satisfaction
by assessing
-
desire to burn your
workplace down.
-
The question, how
likely are you to
-
call the fire department
-
if your workplace
was burning down,
-
is not necessarily measuring
your workplace satisfaction,
-
but it might be
measuring how likely you
-
are to not want it to burn down.
-
But if I want to measure
-
how much I want the
building to burn down,
-
then I need to
reverse code that.
-
If you said, strongly disagree,
-
I would not want to
call a fire department;
-
that would mean now
with reverse coding,
-
it would be strongly agree.
-
I would word the
question instead
-
as I would not be likely to
call the fire department,
-
and then strongly agree.
-
But it'll make more sense once
-
we get to that part
of the chapter.
-
Does it make sense now?
-
Speaker 4: Yeah.
-
Speaker 3: That is
almost word for
-
word what I said. Yes, Hailey.
-
Speaker 4: Just like
what you were talking
-
about the reverse coding,
-
I've taken personality
tests for previous classes.
-
Is that why some questions
are just the same words,
-
but worded backwards on the
big five personality tests?
-
Speaker 3: Exactly.
That's why they're
-
worded backwards because
we want to make sure
-
that you answer consistently
irrespective of
-
how we word the questions
because if I asked you,
-
on a scale of 1-7, how
happy are you from one,
-
no emotions, to seven,
extremely ecstatic.
-
I might also ask you,
-
how unhappy are you from one,
-
to not at all,
-
to seven, no emotions.
-
Then I'd reverse code one of
-
the items to actually
assess happiness.
-
Speaker 4: I've always seen
those questions and I'm like,
-
didn't I just answer
questions exactly like
-
this, but that makes sense.
-
: Rick. Sorry.
-
: Hey, [inaudible] exactly
what you're saying.
-
: That's exactly what I'm
thinking when I'm reading this.
-
: It just clarified something.
-
: It was really good
because all the
-
: time in all these surveys,
-
: I'm going, I've just read
that question, so this helps.
-
Speaker 3: That's the
reason why we do it.
-
We want to make sure that
there's reliability in
-
the scale because if you
-
answer the questions
differently,
-
then that means that
we're not appropriately
-
measuring the construct or we're
-
not measuring it as well
as we thought we would.
-
I said those in words so
you can have that later.
-
What about in journal articles?
-
Rick, you'll probably
read about this.
-
All of you will probably
read about this, but Rick,
-
you were talking
about physiology,
-
and self-report, and
things like that.
-
You'll see the reliability
presented like this.
-
I'm trying to get
my mouse over here.
-
Test-retest reliability,
for instance, 0.83,
-
0.84, 0.6, 0.82, 0.5, 0.54.
-
Hailey, are you looking
at a meta analysis?
-
You might find this too.
-
Speaker 4: I have, I think,
-
two articles that
are meta analysis.
-
Speaker 3: You might
find some values
-
like this for test-retest.
-
This is telling you how
long between the tests.
-
This is a satisfaction with
-
life scale, but
they're different.
-
There's Alfonso and Allison,
-
Pavot et al, Blais et al,
-
Diener et al, and
so on and so forth.
-
If you take the tests
that Alfonso and Allison
-
gave at Time 1 and
two weeks later,
-
the test-retest reliability
is 0.83 with one month.
-
With a Pavot et al, 0.84.
-
Two months, you see
it going down, 0.64.
-
The Diener et al
test-retest reliability
-
is actually higher
at two months.
-
At 10 weeks, of course,
-
it goes down to 0.5,
-
and in four years,
-
it goes down to 0.54.
-
That means that the
scores that you have at
-
Time 1 aren't necessarily as
-
related at Time 2.
Does that make sense?
-
The coefficient Alpha is for
the internal reliability.
-
How good do all of the questions
-
that are in that scale measure
-
the construct that
you're interested in?
-
Here you want a good value.
-
How well does, I'm very happy
-
and I feel really sad,
reverse-coded measure happiness?
-
Here, the coefficient
Alpha is 0.89.
-
That's good because
it's close to one.
-
0.85, still good,
0.79-0.84 still good.
-
All of these are
greater than 0.70,
-
which means that all the items
-
tend to hang together nicely.
-
Does that make sense?
-
That's what you'll see in
-
the text or any
articles that you read.
-
Now we're going to talk about
validity of measurement.
-
We're going to talk about
measurement validity
-
of abstract constructs.
-
We've talked about reliability.
-
Now we're moving on to validity.
-
What is the difference between
-
face validity and
content validity?
-
Does it look like
a good measure?
-
Criterion validity.
-
Does it correlate
with key behaviors.
-
Convergent validity and
discriminate validity.
-
Does a pattern make sense?
-
Relationship between
reliability and validity.
-
What are the three
reliabilities again?
-
Rick. Alicia or whoever
was saying that.
-
Speaker 4: Test-retest. I
keep wanting to say relator.
-
: Interrater.
-
: Interrater. My gosh. I'm
blanking on the third one.
-
Speaker 3: Internal. How do
we measure the strength of
-
those reliabilities?
-
It's a letter.
-
: The coefficient R.
-
Speaker 3: Coefficient
R. The coefficient R,
-
what are the endpoints?
Or what's the range?
-
: Negative one to one
including [inaudible]
-
Speaker 4: Perfect. What
do we want the value
-
to be to be good reliability?
-
We want it to be positive and
closer to one as possible.
-
That's what I've been lecturing
about for the last hour.
-
Also last lecture
and this lecture.
-
I just want to make sure we
are all on the same page.
-
Now we're moving to validity.
-
Let's move on to validity.
-
We talked about
reliability here,
-
so test-retest, internal,
and interrater reliability.
-
Now, we're going to move on to
-
here with construct validity,
-
and we'll talk about all of
these different validities,
-
but we're mostly
going to be talking
-
about construct validity here.
-
We're going to jump into
-
two subjective ways
to assess validity,
-
so face validity and
content validity.
-
Face validity is exactly
what it sounds like.
-
Does your measure look like
what you want to measure?
-
Then content validity
is the measure
-
contains all the parts that
-
your theory says
it should contain.
-
We have different standards for
-
measurements as
physical scientists,
-
so we can use rulers, scales,
-
thermometers to ensure that
-
we're measuring
things like length,
-
weight, and
temperature reliably.
-
However, psychological
scientists
-
are interested in measuring
-
abstract concepts
like self-esteem,
-
or how much you hate
your job, or depression.
-
That's more abstract.
-
Because of that, our
construct validity is a lot
-
more important to
psychological scientists.
-
For people who measure
physical things like
-
weight, well,
construct validity.
-
How would you measure weight?
-
The scale.
-
That's pretty easy, but for
more abstract concepts,
-
construct validity
is super important.
-
Let's jump into face validity
and content validity.
-
Like I said, face
validity is it looks
-
like what you want to measure
and content validity,
-
the measure contains all of
-
the parts your theory
says it should contain.
-
Alicia, what is
your study about?
-
How introversion and
-
extroversion affect
anxiety management.
-
Speaker 3: Having
good validity would
-
be asking questions
related to introversion.
-
Do you enjoy spending
time with others?
-
Do you get energized by spending
-
time with others?
Things like that.
-
It seems to have good face
validity and content validity.
-
Contains all the parts that
-
your theory says
it should contain.
-
With introversion, we're talking
-
about how much time you
like spending with others,
-
so we're asking those questions,
-
but if I ask
questions related to,
-
how much time you spend online,
-
that might not be good content
validity because it's not
-
assessing how much time you
enjoy spending with others.
-
: [inaudible]
-
: It could if you did how
much time you spent on
-
: Facebook talking to people
or whatever the case may be.
-
: Does that make sense, face
-
: validity and content validity?
-
: If I was measuring happiness,
-
: it might be asking questions
about how happy you are,
-
: how sad you are.
-
: It might not have as
-
: good content validity if
I ask how angry you are,
-
: how violent you are.
-
: That might not be a great
measure of happiness.
-
: It might be a great
measure of likelihood to
-
: engage in violence,
but not happiness.
-
Speaker 3: Then we have
criterion validity,
-
and this is when you
want to see whether
-
your variable of interest
correlates with key behaviors.
-
Most psychological
scientists prefer to rely on
-
empirical assessments of
-
validity over
subjective judgments.
-
One way to empirically
assess validity is
-
by examining criterion
validity, which is here.
-
Whether the measure
is related to
-
a concrete outcome that
it should be related to.
-
Let's say you work
for a company that
-
wants to predict how well
-
job applicants do a salespeople.
-
Initially, the company used
-
IQ scores to predict sales
aptitude. Here we go.
-
The criterion
validity. Right here,
-
we're checking how
well aptitude test
-
A relates to sale figures
in thousands of dollars.
-
You wanted your IQ test to
-
predict or relate to a
behavior, sales figures.
-
As you can tell,
-
as aptitude test scores go up,
-
so do sales figures in
dollars. They're, great.
-
We can use IQ to predict how
well our employees will do.
-
Let's say that then they
also have aptitude test B,
-
which is a different IQ test.
-
They're looking at
how that relates to
-
sales figures and what they
-
find because these dots are a
-
lot more scattered
from that line,
-
that aptitude test B
doesn't have as great of
-
a criterion validity
as aptitude test A.
-
That means that aptitude
test B is less likely to
-
predict sales figures
relative to aptitude test A.
-
Is that clear? Just
criterion validity is you're
-
using one measure to
-
predict behaviors or
something another scale.
-
: You're comparing the
measures that you're going to
-
: use to see which
one is best serious.
-
: No, that's good.
-
: For the burning
of the building,
-
: you might want to use
burning of the building scale
-
: to what people actually do
when the building is on fire,
-
: how likely you
are to put the fire
-
: out if there is
an actual fire.
-
: What you find is maybe
-
: people burning saying
that they would burn
-
: a building is not the same
as what they actually do in
-
: a fire so might not have
great criterion validity.
-
: Another way to gather
evidence for criterion validity
-
: is to use a known
groups paradigm.
-
: What is a knowns
group paradigm?
-
: This is when you have
-
: a group that you
know that scores high
-
: on this measure or
on a related measure,
-
: and you create a new measure.
-
: For instance, the Beck
Depression Inventory
-
: is one of the most
popularly used inventory
-
: to measure depression.
-
: But when it first came out,
-
: there were already existing
measures of depression.
-
: If you want to
create a new measure,
-
: you have to have a good
reason to include that measure.
-
: The BEC depression
inventory is
-
: a 21 item, self report scales.
-
: In order to test the
criterion validity
-
: of this depression scale,
-
: you could administer the BDI
-
: to a group of people
with depression,
-
: so are already
in treatment for
-
: depression and a group of
people who aren't depressed.
-
: If you find here,
-
: this is the known group.
-
: You have a group of
people who are depressed.
-
: You have an inventory that
should measure depression.
-
: If you find that depressed
people score higher on
-
: your depression
inventory relative
-
: to those that
are not depressed,
-
: then that tells us that
there's good criterion validity.
-
: The depression
inventory actually
-
: predicts who is depressed
versus who is not.
-
: This is the known
groups method.
-
: Another way is to separate
-
: the depression that people
are experiencing from mild,
-
: moderate, and severe.
-
: This could be the
psychiatrists rating of how
-
: much their patients are
experiencing depression,
-
: and the BDI score,
-
: the BEC depression
inventory should be able
-
: to distinguish between
those who have mild,
-
: moderate and severe
depression, which happens here.
-
: Severe people are
experiencing the most amount of
-
: BDI relative to those who are
-
: moderate and mild depression.
-
: Is that clear?
-
: Do you guys
-
: like me to use
the weird examples.
-
: Example. We're good.
-
: I was fine. The burning
-
: the house down or burning
your place of employment,
-
: you might have pyromaniacs and
-
: measure whether
pyromaniacs would
-
: score higher on the
likelihood to burn
-
: the employment
down relative to
-
: those who are not pyromaniacs,
-
: and you would
want people who are
-
: pyromaniacs to score higher on
-
: likelihood of burning place
of employment down relative
-
: to those that
aren't pyromaniacs.
-
: That would be a
good measure of fire.
-
: I actually like my job.
-
: I don't know why I
came up with that.
-
Speaker 4: It's very dark.
-
Speaker 3: It is
very dark. I don't
-
know I came up
with that example.
-
Perfect. Does that make sense?
-
Your measure should be able to
-
predict likelihood of starting
-
a fire and the degree
-
to which you would be
likely to start a fire.
-
Pyromaniac should
be high here and
-
then mild pyromaniacs versus
-
moderate pyromaniacs
versus severe pyromaniacs,
-
it should also be
able to predict
-
the extent to which
people are pyromaniacs.
-
That's one way to establish
-
whether your scale
is good or not.
-
We can also use known
groups evidence
-
for criterion validity.
-
This is Diener's subjective
well being scale.
-
Remember, Diener created
this to measure happiness.
-
We're using it for different
groups of individuals.
-
What we are using is
American college students,
-
French Canadian
college students,
-
Korean university students,
printing trade workers,
-
veterans Affairs
hospital patients,
-
abused women, and
male prison innates.
-
These are the references where
they got the scores from.
-
What the Diener's subjective
well being scale would want
-
is scores that map on to
-
these different
measures of happiness.
-
Does that make sense? When
you measure happiness
-
with Pavot and Diener measure,
-
you have a 6.4 of
-
happiness for American
college students.
-
With Blais et al, you have 6.1,
-
for Suh study, it's 5.8,
-
for George, it's 6.0,
-
for veterans Affairs
hospital inpatients
-
with Frisch, it is 5.6,
-
for Fisher at 7.4,
-
and that's the
standard deviation.
-
Abused women is 20.7,
-
and male prison inmates
with Joy is 12.3.
-
Just by looking at this,
-
do you think that
-
these measures are okay
measures of happiness?
-
For instance, who is the
happiest based on these values?
-
Speaker 4: Printing
trade workers.
-
Speaker 3: Printing
trade workers,
-
their jobs are
probably pretty chill.
-
American college students or
-
these college students might
still be super stressed out,
-
but they're still
relatively happy.
-
What about male prison inmates
-
and Veterans Affair
hospital inpatients?
-
They're pretty low on happiness.
-
It tracks so it does seem
-
to accurately measure happiness.
-
Because the actual values
-
of happiness are tracking
what we would expect,
-
then we can say that the
subjective well-being scale
-
would have high
criterion validity.
-
It actually measures
what it's intended to
-
measure. Does that make sense?
-
Speaker 4: Yeah, I
can read that one.
-
Speaker 3: This one's
very clear. Now we
-
have convergent validity
and discriminant validity.
-
This is concerned
with whether there is
-
a meaningful pattern of
similarities and differences.
-
Convergent, as you might guess,
-
is how well does the BDI,
-
Beck Depression
Inventory relate to
-
an already existing
inventory for depression,
-
which is the Center
for Epidemiologic
-
: Studies Depression Scale.
-
Speaker 3: It's not hard work.
-
The Center for Epidemiologic,
-
no I can't say it.
-
You know what I'm trying to say.
-
Studies Depression Scale,
-
and these have a correlation
coefficient of 0.68.
-
I'm just going to
call it the CES-D.
-
When the Beck Depression
Inventory came on the scene,
-
this was the most popularly
used measure of depression.
-
But the BDI needs to
-
be similar or converge
-
on an already existing
measure of depression,
-
but it also needs to be slightly
-
unique because if it's
exactly the same,
-
then why are we
creating a new scale?
-
Right here, let's say
-
we have people that we
know are depressed,
-
and they take both
CES-D and the BDI.
-
What we see is that the
scores do tend to converge.
-
As people increase in BDI,
-
they also tend to
increase in CES-D.
-
But it's not a perfect
relationship, and that's fine.
-
We don't actually want
it to be perfect.
-
Now, discriminant
validity is how
-
well and how different is this
scale from other measures.
-
A measure should
correlate less strongly
-
with measures of
different constructs.
-
In other words, there
must be differences.
-
The Beck Depression Inventory
is supposed to measure
-
psychological well being or
-
non-well being and not
-
necessarily physical
health problems.
-
If this has good
discriminate validity,
-
which it does, that
means that your BDI,
-
Beck Depression
Inventory should be not
-
related to physical
health problems
-
and that's what we find.
-
As BDI increases, it doesn't
-
necessarily predict how many
health problems you'll have.
-
Convergent validity
is how well does
-
your measure map onto
already existing measures,
-
and discriminant
validity is about how
-
much your measure correlates
-
with measures of
different constructs.
-
Think of convergent as same and
-
discriminant as different
with different scales.
-
Any questions here?
Then the relationship
-
between reliability
and validity.
-
We talked a lot about
reliability and validity.
-
Remember that just
because a measure is
-
reliable doesn't mean
it's a valid measure.
-
For instance, I could measure
my cat's head every day,
-
and I'll have a really reliable
measure of my cat's head,
-
but that might not be
a valid measure of IQ.
-
Head size might not be
a valid measure of IQ,
-
meaning that it doesn't
actually measure IQ.
-
It's reliable, but
it's not valid.
-
Reliability is more of how well
-
a measure correlates
with itself.
-
Invalidity is how
well a measure is
-
associated with something else.
-
A measure can be less
valid than it is reliable,
-
but it cannot be more
valid than it is reliable.
-
For instance, a measure can
be less valid than it is
-
reliable so measuring
my cat's head
-
might not be related to its IQ,
-
so it's less valid
than it's reliable,
-
but it can't be more
valid than it's reliable.
-
IQ scores cannot be a
more valid measure of
-
head circumference relative to
-
how reliable the head
circumference and IQ is.
-
Reliability is necessary but
not sufficient for validity.
-
These are just things
to consider as you're
-
thinking about your projects.
-
There will be some checks and
-
balances that you'll have
to go through when you're
-
thinking about validity
and reliability.
-
You'll want your scales to be
-
reliable and you'll also
want them to be valid.
-
Reliable means that the scales
-
actually measure what you
want them to measure,
-
and validity is how well
-
that measure is associated
with something else.
-
For instance, Hailey, if you're
-
interested in job persistence,
-
you want the measure of job
persistence to actually
-
predict the people being
-
in their jobs maybe
five years later.
-
That would be the validity.
-
That is it for today.
-
Any questions?
-
We're going through
a lot of chapters,
-
and I think it's good
that we're actually
-
keeping on track.
-
But if you don't have any more
questions, then that's it.
-
Enjoy your weekend.
Alicia, if you
-
: want to stay. Thank you.
-
Speaker 3: Thank you.
-
: Thanks.
-
Speaker 4: Thank you.
-
: Thank you.
-
: Let's see. I stopped
recording. I think I did.