-
Can you hear us, Rick?
-
Yeah, I can now. I forgot
-
I didn't have on my headphones.
-
I thought it might
be helpful to go
-
over some of the assignments
that are due next week.
-
Can everyone see
the Canvas page?
-
Yes. Great.
-
I opened up Module 4,
-
which will include
the introduction,
-
and I just wanted to give you
-
a brief overview of that today.
-
I have a video
-
I recorded for another
research methods class,
-
and I can put that up as well,
-
but we'll be talking
about it today.
-
That peer review is not
happening. Here we go.
-
There's an introduction
template that I wrote that
-
could help you write the paper.
-
I also want you to check out
the actual submission link.
-
Within the submission link,
-
you'll get more information
-
about how to write
the introduction.
-
But I think the most
important component
-
of this will be to take
a look at the rubric.
-
Because when you take
a look at the rubric,
-
you'll be able to tell what
-
I'm placing most of
the attention on.
-
This whole thing is out of 50,
-
but in reality,
-
the introduction will
only be worth 25 points,
-
so you have to divide it by two.
-
As you can see,
-
just by looking at
the point values,
-
what really matters is
-
the main body of the intro,
so the literature review.
-
Most of you are
already working on
-
that with your Assignment 1B,
-
looking for different
articles that might be
-
relevant to your
topic of interest.
-
But let's go through
the opening paragraph.
-
First, you have to put a title
-
before your first paragraph.
-
That's half a point.
Should I make it
-
bigger or is it too small?
-
A little bigger would be better.
-
I'll do that. Give me a second.
-
How about there?
-
Is that good? Title before
-
the first paragraph
is half a point,
-
a catchy opening hook to
grab the reader's attention.
-
Hailey, what's your topic?
-
It's about sexism in
-
the workplace for women in
higher positions of power.
-
It could be like a catchy hook.
-
It could be cheesy right
now. It doesn't matter.
-
[LAUGHTER] This is
going to be really bad,
-
but I just thought of it
-
like imagine you're
a woman at work
-
and one of your male colleagues
-
smacks your butt on your way in.
-
Though this is less likely to
be a reality for women now.
-
This is really common, like
in the '50s and the '60s.
-
Although overt sexual harassment
-
is less likely to happen now
to women in the workplace,
-
sexual harassment is
still an issue for women,
-
and then you can keep talking.
-
If I read that, I'd
be like, my God, no.
-
I want to know what happens
to this guy if he smacked
-
my butt. Something like that.
-
State the big picture or
problem and its importance.
-
Although sexual harassment might
-
not be as overt
as it was before,
-
women in the workplace
-
still experience
sexual harassment.
-
That is the importance
in the big picture.
-
Frames the context of the
story to be discussed.
-
It depends on what exactly
you'll be looking at.
-
Hailey, what exactly
will you be looking at?
-
Like perceptions of sexual
harassment or the behaviors?
-
I really want to look at how it
-
affects the women and
-
their ability to continue
on in their career choice.
-
Got it. Maybe you'll
-
recruit women and you'll
have them recall a time
-
when they've experienced
sexual harassment
-
or imagine a scenario
where they experienced
-
sexual harassment and then
-
assess the likelihood
that they'd
-
be able to continue
in their job,
-
that they'd want to persist,
-
that they feel
like they're safe,
-
that they could seek mentors
and so on and so forth.
-
When you're framing the context
-
of the story to be discussed,
-
you're going to talk about
what your interest is.
-
Then you'll provide a brief
outline of the paper.
-
In this research paper,
-
I will be discussing
historical articles
-
on women's experiences of
-
sexual harassment
in the workplace,
-
the types of sexual harassment
-
that are typically
likely to occur,
-
other people's perceptions of
-
sexual harassment
in the workplace,
-
and finally, women's
likelihood of
-
persisting in the
workplace after
-
experiencing a trauma like
-
sexual harassment
in the workplace.
-
That summary is
basically going to
-
be the four or five articles
you're going to talk about.
-
You're just talking
about the main points,
-
and then boom, you're done
with the opening paragraph.
-
The problems are things that you
-
might get dock points on.
-
I would try to just get
as many points here.
-
I won't really dock points here,
-
but I will note these as
-
problems so that you can
work on it in the future.
-
If your introduction appears
-
instead of the paper's title,
-
no, I want your title.
-
If you don't introduce properly
-
before getting into details,
-
so don't have a
catchy opening hook,
-
that is not great.
-
The statement of the problem
is too brief or lacking.
-
Does not provide a
framework for the story,
-
so you're not telling
me what you will
-
tell me in the future
or how you're going to
-
outline or make an argument for
-
your research paper
and other things.
-
It could be like citing
issues and things like that.
-
Then for the literature review,
-
this is going to be the
bulk of your paper.
-
These will be like the
four or five studies that
-
you'll be talking
about that you may
-
have already done for
your Assignment 1B.
-
I want you to describe the
relevant parts of each study.
-
Hailey, have you read any
papers for your study yet?
-
I haven't completed
the assignment yet,
-
but I have five articles
picked out for it.
-
Cool.
-
One is how male allies can
help women in the workplace.
-
That one just gives an
account from male allies
-
in the workplace and how they
-
have helped in the past and how
-
the women want them to
help in the future.
-
I think that would be great.
-
It might not be one of
the first paragraphs.
-
It might be one of the
ending paragraphs,
-
but I would want you to evaluate
-
the importance of each study
-
and describe the relevant parts.
-
How did they collect
data from these males?
-
How did they identify allies?
-
What behaviors did they
describe as allied behavior?
-
How did women perceive
these behaviors?
-
That would be good.
I also want you to
-
tie findings together and
exploit common themes.
-
Across all of the five papers
-
that you're reading, of course,
-
they will be distinct,
but they will share
-
a similar theme of women
-
experiencing sexual
harassment in the workplace.
-
I want you to find similarities
and differences between
-
them and also to cite
-
your paper or your
articles in APA format.
-
This is why the citing
paper comes into play.
-
I'll be grading that today.
-
Great. The problems would
-
be an insufficient
literature review.
-
Maybe you just talk
about two studies.
-
This happened before you give
me the title of the paper,
-
but you don't really tell me
what the paper was about.
-
That really doesn't
give me an idea
-
of what research you've read,
-
that leads you to
ask this question.
-
I don't want all the details
about the study methods.
-
I don't want you to tell me they
-
recruited males from Portland,
-
Oregon on Tuesday and
-
Thursday because those are
the days that males go out.
-
I don't want to know
all that information.
-
I just want to know general.
-
You don't discuss the
importance of each study,
-
so, of course,
describe the study.
-
Main claims and supporting
-
evidence not clear
from citations.
-
Again, don't just put
the title of the study.
-
Give me some summary.
-
Summarizes article after article
-
instead of synthesizing
research findings.
-
I do want you to
include a summary,
-
but you should also have
-
a common thread
throughout and talk
-
about how the
articles inform one
-
another and how they
-
help you form your
research question.
-
Improper APA
citations and other.
-
Of course, APA is going to
be important throughout.
-
After you write the bulk of
-
your research or your
literature review,
-
you'll have a
paragraph identifying
-
gaps to motivate your study.
-
For instance, for Hailey,
it would be something like,
-
although research has examined
how male allies can help
-
women experiencing
sexual harassment
-
in the workplace,
limited research.
-
This might not be true,
but I'm just spitballing.
-
Limited research
has investigated
-
how sexual harassment affects
women's persistence in
-
the workplace and what
strategies women can
-
engage in for themselves
-
to deal with sexual
harassment in the workplace.
-
You'd compare and contrast
findings across studies.
-
This is where you can
include a sentence or
-
two for each of
-
the studies that you've
already discussed.
-
Talk about the limitations of
-
previous work and gaps
in the literature.
-
Sometimes allies are people
-
who exhibit sexual harassment
towards other people,
-
so maybe talk about how
allies are important,
-
but they can also be
-
perpetrators of sexual
harassment, etc.
-
Reiterate your main claims and
-
outline evidence to set
-
the context for
your current study,
-
and have APA
citations throughout.
-
If you don't compare and
contrast, that's not great,
-
if you don't critically evaluate
-
the limitations of the
work, that's not great,
-
if you don't summarize
everything that
-
you've talked about so far
-
before jumping into
the current study,
-
that's also not great.
-
Then your final paragraph will
-
be talking about your
study more specifically.
-
Hailey, the purpose
of my study is to
-
see how women experience,
-
sexual harassment
in the workplace,
-
and how that might
affect their persistence
-
in mostly male dominated
job or something.
-
Describe how your
experimental variables
-
will be manipulated
and measured.
-
Participants will be assigned
to one of two conditions.
-
In condition 1, participants
will be asked to either
-
recall or imagine a scenario
-
where they experience
sexual harassment,
-
or in the second scenario,
-
participants will be asked to
-
recall one of their job
experiences just generally.
-
Then all participants
will complete
-
measures of persistence
in the workplace,
-
and other things, whatever
your dependent variables are.
-
Then your testable hypotheses.
-
I predict that people in
-
the sexual harassment condition
-
will exhibit lower levels of
-
persistence in the
workplace compared to women
-
who describe your
typical job scenario.
-
The problems might be
lacking theoretical support,
-
but that's where
your intro comes in.
-
That's where you're
talking about
-
the articles that actually
-
helped you come up
with this theory.
-
I do want you to talk about
your IVs and your DVs,
-
so maybe you'll talk
about persistence.
-
Persistence will be measured by
-
the everyday persistence
scale created
-
by Chatterley and
colleagues in 2020.
-
If you don't tell me
what your hypothesis is,
-
then that's not great.
-
If I don't know what
your hypothesis is,
-
or it seems unclear because
-
of the lack of information
from prior studies,
-
it's also not great, and if it's
-
unclear how your study will test
-
hypotheses, it's also not great.
-
Let's say Hailey
said something like,
-
"I'm interested in
seeing persistent rates
-
among women who experienced
sexual harassment.
-
I'm going to recruit
10 male participants,
-
and I'm going to ask
them about how many
-
times they've sexually
harassed a woman."
-
That's not answering the
question that she posed.
-
That's completely different.
-
Now your references.
-
Again, references are
pretty important APA style.
-
Having a title that
says references,
-
using APA style,
-
everything you cited in
-
your paper appears in
the references and
-
having at least four
papers that you can cite.
-
Of course, you should
already have five with
-
assignment 1B, so
that should be great.
-
The problems could be it's
not formatted correctly,
-
APA style isn't followed
or used inconsistently.
-
That's why I'm going
to try to get you
-
feedback by the end of today,
-
if not the end of
tomorrow on your site me,
-
so that you know if
-
you did anything wrong,
what you can do to improve.
-
They have to be in
alphabetical order.
-
If you use less
than four sources,
-
you will be docked points,
-
does not match the text.
-
Hailey, if you cited Chatterley
and colleagues in 2020,
-
but then you use Tingle
and colleagues 2020,
-
then I would be
like, "Well, what
-
happened to the other citation?"
-
Too many secondary
sources cited.
-
So this would be using
data from CNN to see
-
how many women experience
-
sexual harassment and not
enough empirical articles.
-
You can use secondary sources,
-
but try not to use
more than two.
-
Then just overall format,
-
your organization, your
grammar, your spelling,
-
the length is sufficient for
-
the full picture of the problem,
-
your margins are good,
-
your double spacing is good,
-
your indentation's good,
-
you're using 12-point font,
-
and you have page numbers.
-
You don't start out
broadly and narrowly.
-
Of course, Hailey
with your paper,
-
you're going to be like, "Women
-
used to experience
sexual harassment."
-
That'll be really broad,
-
and then you'll go very
detailed because you'll talk
-
about your experiment
specifically.
-
It is a lot easier
to paraphrase.
-
If you're having trouble
paraphrasing something,
-
let me know and I can help
-
using active rather
than passive voice.
-
Don't give personal opinions.
-
If your margins
are inconsistent,
-
your double spacing
is inconsistent,
-
consistent font is not used,
-
page number is inconsistent
in the wrong place,
-
you spell check isn't used,
-
they're choppy transitions,
-
they're fragmented sentences,
-
length is insufficient to
-
capture the breath of a problem.
-
After all that, you
can get 50 points,
-
you divide it by 20, and
that'll be worth 25 points.
-
Just to let you know,
-
I do have a way of grading
all of these papers.
-
The first draft is worth less,
-
and then the second
draft is worth more than
-
the final draft is
worth the most.
-
I do that because I know you're
-
still learning how
to write a paper,
-
so if you do get docked
points on the intro,
-
I want you to be able to gain
-
those points by doing
better on the second draft.
-
The points that you
might lose here aren't
-
as meaningful as they would be
-
for the second or
the third draft.
-
Once you write the second draft,
-
it will include a revised intro,
-
and then your first draft
of your method and results.
-
Then your final draft will
include your revised intro,
-
revised method, and results
and a discussion section.
-
You're always going
to get feedback,
-
and my goal is
-
not for you to get it
perfect the first time,
-
because I remember this
was really hard for
-
me when I learned how
to write a paper,
-
so I want to see improvement.
-
I don't expect everyone
to get a perfect score,
-
but I would like to
see improvement.
-
Are there any questions here?
-
I will be uploading a
sample paper as well,
-
but I did want to just give
you a brief introduction.
-
But, how do I write
an introduction?
-
I feel the rubric is
good for telling you,
-
like, "These are
the things that you
-
should and shouldn't
do to get the points.
-
But I wrote this template
-
that you can use when you're
writing your introduction."
-
Let me open it up
in Word document
-
because this looks weird.
-
I'm going to stop the share,
-
and let me get my
Word document open.
-
That's a lot of information
I've thrown at you-all.
-
Any questions so far?
-
I really want to read
Hailey's paper now.
-
Let me share the screen again.
-
Great, and I'm going
to make it bigger.
-
I created this document to
-
help you on the
upcoming assignment.
-
You can read all of this.
-
But the author of your textbook
-
recommends you write
the main points of
-
an argument and individual
research findings
-
on index card,
-
so then you can
rearrange them to
-
figure out what makes
the most sense.
-
Let me get the sun out of
-
my face so to figure out
what makes the most sense.
-
You can also do this by
writing the topic sentence
-
of each paragraph in this
Word document and then
-
finding support for
your arguments from
-
the five or more articles you've
-
reviewed for Assignment 1B.
-
Things to keep in mind.
Your intro should be
-
between five to maybe
seven paragraphs.
-
That might be anywhere
between 3-5 pages,
-
so it shouldn't be that intense.
-
The most difficult part
is finding how to weave
-
the entire narrative through
-
the article summaries
that you have.
-
The first sentence
of each paragraph
-
should be a topic sentence.
-
For instance, if Hailey is
-
going to be talking
about male allies,
-
so she could say
something like, men,
-
not only women can provide
support for those who are
-
experiencing sexual
harassment and
-
also to prevent sexual
harassment from re-occurring.
-
Then you'll go into that study.
-
The topic sentence makes a claim
-
that the following sentences
-
support using research
findings as evidence.
-
Chatterly, is it Chatterly?
-
Chatterley and
colleagues conducted
-
a study where men were recruited
and so on and so forth,
-
and then you go on to the
summary of the study.
-
Respected and
experienced researchers,
-
researchers do not
use many, if any,
-
quotes when writing,
so try to paraphrase.
-
You can always paraphrase.
-
When discussing the
findings of past research,
-
only provide as
much detail about
-
the researcher's method as
-
is necessary for your reader
to understand the results.
-
I don't want how many
people they collected,
-
I don't want when
they collected it.
-
I just want maybe to know that
all the participants were
-
self-identified as male and
-
what questions they were
asked about allied behaviors.
-
If I'm curious about the
methodological details,
-
I think it's really interesting,
-
I can always look up
the information myself
-
by using the references
that you've cited.
-
Sure. Go ahead.
-
Well, this is all for
the intro, so okay.
-
Yeah. Do you have a question
about it, or is it?
-
It just caught me right there.
-
I just go, okay,
-
that's a lot, but I'm
following it. Sure.
-
It is a lot. I feel
-
like I would rather give you
-
more information
than not enough.
-
This is helpful. A
guideline is what I need.
-
Yeah. This is two pages long,
-
so I'll show you what I created.
-
There's just like things
to keep in mind generally,
-
and then I have paragraph
outlines that you can use.
-
They can end each paragraph
with a sentence communicating
-
the implications of a finding
-
you discussed in the paragraph.
-
I want you to answer
the question,
-
so what about the
research you summarized?
-
This tells the reader what
-
they should have taken
away from that paragraph.
-
The research from
Chatterley and colleagues
-
suggests that males are
-
important sources of preventing
-
future instances of
sexual harassment.
-
These are outlines
for your paper.
-
You could use this. Begin your
introduction with a hook,
-
something that is general.
-
What did I say? Imagine
you walk into work and
-
your male colleague
slaps your behind.
-
Though this might not represent
-
the sexual harassment that
women experience in the work.
-
Women do still experience
sexual harassment.
-
The present study will
-
explore the ways in
which women experience
-
sexual harassment
and the consequences
-
of sexual harassment for
persistence in the workplace.
-
First, I will outline.
-
Next, we will discuss
-
and then following,
blah. Then finally,
-
you talk about your
actual experiment.
-
First, I said,
-
then your first
paragraph follows.
-
That could be the
first paragraph in
-
your intro. Does
that make sense?
-
It's something really broad.
-
You talk about your
present study,
-
and then you give an outline
-
of what you'll be talking about.
-
You're telling me what
main points I should get.
-
The next sentence
should introduce how
-
your hook relates to
your research question.
-
Though this might not represent
-
the sexual harassment that women
-
experience in the workplace.
-
Today, women do still
experience sexual harassment.
-
The next few sentences should
discuss existing research
-
broadly and also outline
-
the limits of the
current research.
-
How your research question
will address the existing gap.
-
These sentences should
foreshadow what you will be
-
discussing in the
following paragraphs.
-
The present study will
explore the ways in
-
which women experience
sexual harassment and
-
the consequences of
sexual harassment
-
for persistence
in the workplace.
-
First, you're going
to outline law.
-
Next, we'll discuss
that following,
-
blah and then finally,
-
I will explore how these
studies tie together.
-
But I'd be more specific to
inform the current study.
-
The final sentence should
be about your study
-
that outline your IV
and your levels of it,
-
your dependent variable and
-
your predictions, and
so on and so forth.
-
I have a question.
-
Yes, go for it.
-
About the existing gap.
-
Could you give an example
or explain a little bit?
-
I missed really.
-
Sure.
-
How your research question
will address that?
-
Limited research to date
has directly examined
-
the consequences of
sexual harassment
-
for persistence
in the workplace.
-
You want to show work that
needs to be done, but hasn't.
-
You can say that about
mostly any study
-
because there are specific
studies that are out there,
-
but there are always ways
to make studies unique.
-
Right there at that point,
-
you want to state how years
is going to be unique?
-
How this study is
going to fill that.
-
Yeah. Rick, your study is
about COVID-19, right?
-
Well, yeah, depression due to
isolation from COVID, yeah.
-
Right. Your intro could
be something like,
-
in 2019 and before,
-
most people would say that
-
they'd love to stay home
and watch Netflix all day.
-
In 2020, that became a reality.
-
But what people didn't
anticipate is that they would
-
experience increasing
levels of loneliness.
-
The COVID-19 pandemic
was unprecedented,
-
and people experience
increased people
-
may have experienced
increased sense
-
of loneliness and depression.
-
Almost no research to date
-
exists to examine
how pandemics and
-
quarantining has effects on
-
people's emotional and
psychological state.
-
The current research
will explore law.
-
Because that's, like,
brand new research.
-
Well, I did found
some good ones.
-
Yeah, so there have been papers
-
that have been published
really quickly.
-
But prior to those papers,
-
there's been really
nothing because we weren't
-
doing this research
during the Spanish flu.
-
But those statements can be
made almost about any paper.
-
Thus, the present
study will explore how
-
these studies tie together
to inform the current study.
-
This would be a lot better.
-
This is just like
me spit-balling.
-
Also, you should know,
writing is hard.
-
This is not something
I would submit,
-
but this would be
something that I'm like,
-
Okay, that's a good start,
-
and then I would
go back and edit.
-
I wouldn't anticipate writing
-
this paper the day before
and submitting it.
-
I would maybe set a timer
for 30 minutes and say,
-
All right, I'm just going
to outline this paper.
-
No matter how crap it is,
-
it's still better than nothing,
-
and you can always work
with it to make it better.
-
Okay, so that's the
intro paragraph.
-
Then I want you to
support your argument.
-
Write the support for
your argument statement
-
as the beginning
of the paragraph.
-
Based on the literature
review you did,
-
what articles are finding,
support your argument?
-
Write a few sentences based
on these articles here,
-
reiterate your point, and create
-
a bridge between this
paragraph and the next.
-
Repeat until your argument
is clearly fleshed out.
-
Not to put you on
the spot, Hailey,
-
but I am putting you on spot.
-
Give me some titles of the
papers that you've read.
-
Okay. One is a
confirmatory study
-
of the relations between
workplace sexism,
-
sense of belonging,
mental health,
-
and job satisfaction among
-
women in male-dominated
industries.
-
Give me a sec.
-
Workplace harassment,
belonging, and what else?
-
Mental health and
job satisfaction.
-
Okay, and what's another one?
-
Hold on, women and male.
-
Another one is harmful
workplace experiences
-
and women's occupational
well-being, a meta-analysis.
-
You know what a
meta-analysis is?
-
Well, from what I've read,
-
isn't it just like the author
-
of that article is
-
taking findings from other
studies and kind of,
-
like, compiling it into one?
-
It's not his personal research.
-
It's him comparing a
bunch of other research.
-
Yeah, which might be helpful
-
because you'll be able to read
-
like 20 research articles
in that one article.
-
Yes, that's good. Any other?
-
Yeah, women and women of
color and leadership,
-
complexity, identity,
and intersectionality.
-
Okay, so let's just
go with these three.
-
You could say something one of
-
your paragraphs following
the intro could be
-
sexual harassment may lead to
-
decreased levels of belonging,
-
job satisfaction, and
occupational well-being.
-
Then I talk about
these two articles.
-
This could be two paragraphs.
-
These two articles I
would talk about here.
-
Maybe what I would
do is I would make
-
that a subheading and
make that a subheading.
-
So that will bolster
your argument there.
-
This may be particularly true
-
for women in
male-dominated fields.
-
Maybe that could be
the second paragraph.
-
Wow.
-
Does that make sense? Yeah.
-
Yeah.
-
Okay. The first sentence
-
of the second paragraph
-
might be something more
specific than that,
-
but that could be
-
your first sentence for
the first paragraph,
-
something like this for
the second paragraph.
-
These are the articles
that you will
-
use to support those statements
-
, and so on and so forth.
-
Then the third paragraph
could be something like women
-
with multiple stigmatized
identities may experience
-
the negative effects of
workplace harassment.
-
Seeing multiple may compound
-
the negative effects of
workplace harassment
-
for women or something.
-
Then you'll talk about this:
-
women and women of
color in leadership.
-
Then your fourth paragraph might
-
be something about allies
-
and preventing future
sexual harassment.
-
Then your final
paragraph, there you go.
-
I outlined your paper, Hailey.
It doesn't have to be that.
-
You can rearrange it,
-
but does that make sense Alicia?
-
I can do this for
anyone else's paper.
-
I'm just picking on Hailey.
-
If you want me to help, I can
do this for your paper too.
-
Then your final paragraph,
-
you'll summarize the arguments,
-
points listed in
Paragraphs 2 and 5.
-
As stated earlier,
sexual harassment
-
has negative
implications for women.
-
Women who experience sexual
harassment experience
-
lower levels of belonging
and job satisfaction;
-
citation, lower levels of
-
occupational
well-being; citation.
-
Further, those who possess
-
multiple stigmatized identities,
-
so on and so forth.
-
Then allies may help
-
prevent future sexual harassment
and so on and so forth.
-
However, limited work has
examined how sexual harassment
-
can affect not only
job satisfaction,
-
but also persistence.
-
In the current study,
-
I will examine how
sexual harassment
-
affects persistence among
college-aged women,
-
women of different ethnicities,
whatever you want.
-
You can talk about your sample.
-
I will manipulate sexual
harassment by blah, blah, blah.
-
I will measure persistence
-
by using the everyday workplace.
-
This doesn't exist by
the way. I don't know.
-
Persistent scale;
Chatterley and Tingle,
-
2020 which includes items like,
-
"I want to burn my
office down," on
-
a scale from one
strongly disagree
-
to seven, strongly agree.
-
Just to give me a sense of
what the scale measures.
-
"I often daydream about
burning my office down."
-
Then that's the end.
That's your intro.
-
There it is.
-
Well, does that make sense?
-
Alicia? Does that make sense?
-
I'm looking at it, but I just
want to say it out loud.
-
On the final sentence,
-
you're going to bring in
-
your references
and say what they
-
offered like a summary thing.
-
Mm-hmm. Then finally,
you're going to say,
-
"I predict that women who are
-
in Condition 1 will want to burn
-
their office down more
than those who are in
-
Condition 2," even though that's
-
not what you were
originally measuring.
-
I messed up. That
should be persistence.
-
I'm not burning
your office down.
-
But you get what I'm saying.
-
The last part, this is
the reaction I am going.
-
Here's the questions
that haven't
-
been asked or answered
or something.
-
Yeah. Hailey, I
can send you this.
-
I'm not sure how
helpful it'll be.
-
But anyone else who
comes to these meetings,
-
I'm more than happy
to use your study.
-
Perfect. Does that make it
less intimidating I guess?
-
I created this
just for your sake
-
and also for my sake because
I want better papers.
-
Then your support
for your argument.
-
This will be Paragraphs 2-5
-
and then your final paragraph;
-
Paragraph 6, the end. That's it.
-
Also, remember I do offer
extra credit for visiting
-
the writing center
and you can get
-
extra credit for every time
that you submit a paper.
-
For the intro; for
the method and
-
results and for the discussion,
-
so schedule an appointment
today if you want to get
-
that extra credit and
-
they'll also help you
write your paper.
-
On that should we state,
-
is it the timing of
when our thing is
-
before the paper
or should we state
-
in our thing with
-
the writing center what we
want to go over them about?
-
Do you mean, should you schedule
-
your appointment before
the assignments due?
-
No, just to get credit for
what we're going over.
-
It's just the timing of
the the tutor assigned.
-
Yeah, you can go over anything.
-
You can go over APA formatting,
-
you can go over making
your logical argument.
-
You can go over
anything you want.
-
I just want you to visit
the writing center
-
and make use of the
resources we have available.
-
I wasn't able to get in touch
with Emily for some reason.
-
Was that who you were
about to say we also have?
-
Yes, Emily. I'll reach out
to her. Did you email her?
-
Do you have that information?
-
I will get that information
-
and send it out to
everyone because that's
-
a tutor that we have
that's separate from
-
the writing center and
she's experienced in 301,
-
so she can help with that.
-
I need to talk with her;
-
it's getting deep
on the stuff here,
-
so just to go over stuff
-
and I feel more
comfortable in it.
-
A few things. Appreciate
that. Thank you.
-
Of course, yeah. I'll
send it out to the group.
-
I wrote it down. Alicia,
how are you feeling?
-
I'm feeling okay. I've been
having a little bit of
-
trouble with finding studies.
-
I think it's because I'm not
-
sure specific keywords to
-
use or what to
actually look for.
-
Has anyone else been
having trouble with that?
-
Hailey, it seems like you've
got it all down. Rick?
-
My keywords, I think I had
them right off the bat.
-
Well, depression due
to COVID isolation.
-
It got me some real
pinpoint stuff.
-
They're using different
methods so I can cross.
-
It's nice. There's
one self survey and
-
then there's statistical
and physiological also.
-
It's all great.
-
Alicia, you scheduled an
appointment with me. Right?
-
I have not.
-
Okay. Do you have
time after class?
-
We might not go the full
hour so I can help you.
-
Yeah.
-
Okay. Anyone else?
If you want to help,
-
schedule an appointment
or stay after class,
-
and I will go over those things.
-
I do want to finish the lecture
-
today. Everyone
take a deep breath.
-
The intro will be fine.
-
I need to take a deep breath
too because I feel like
-
I talk so much that
I forget to breathe.
-
The intro will be fine.
-
I'm more than happy to meet with
-
you during my office hours.
-
I still have office
hours this week.
-
I have advising hours
that no one is coming to.
-
You can come to those
advising hours too.
-
I can make time
to meet with you.
-
I'm more than happy to help you.
-
I love research especially
when it's not mine.
-
When it's my research,
I'm just like,
-
"I don't want to do it,' but
I love helping other people.
-
Let me know. Alright, so
let me get my PowerPoint.
-
We're almost done
with the PowerPoint.
-
Where were we?
-
Share screen.
-
Yes. I believe yes,
that is what I want.
-
Because I just got
my external monitor
-
back. Thank goodness.
-
We're going to continue
with Chapter five.
-
If you have a question,
-
just holla because I can't see
-
your faces now for some reason.
-
Identifying good measurement.
-
I do want to see
your faces, though.
-
What do I do? I'm
going to put here.
-
I can't see. Oh, man. I
can't see your faces.
-
Just speak out if you know
-
the answers to the
questions I'm going to ask.
-
Can you see the screen
-
without the notes or
can you see the notes?
-
I just see you.
-
Yeah, right now, it's basics.
-
I see the school.
-
Oh, my bad. Oh, man. Let's see.
-
There they are.
-
Now.
-
Perfect.
-
Now I can see you.
-
We talked about three
types of reliability.
-
What are they?
-
I didn't ask
questions last time,
-
but I'll do it this time.
-
The three types.
-
Alicia. Anyone else?
-
I don't know where
I'm at. Oh, my God.
-
Yes, Hailey.
-
Test reliability, inter-rater
-
reliability, and
internal reliability.
-
Perfect. I won't make you
answer what they are.
-
But Rick or Alicia
or anyone else,
-
do you remember what
those mean? Test, retest.
-
When you test or do, I
want to call it a study.
-
I don't know if that's the
right word, but basically,
-
do it at one point.
-
We're looking at test-retest
reliability for IQ.
-
For IQ? Say, today
you test someone's IQ
-
and they get a specific answer,
-
and then say next
week you test them
-
again and compare the results.
-
Good test for test
reliability would tell us
-
what about the IQ scores?
-
That it's an accurate
representation of
-
their IQ and it's
reliable data to use.
-
Close. Rick or Hailey?
-
I think Meghan
raised their hand.
-
Oh, Meghan, go for it.
I didn't see your hand.
-
It would be consistent,
-
so it would be around
the same quantitatively?
-
Yes, perfect. Let's
say at Time 1,
-
you get an IQ score
of 125 and Time 2,
-
you get an IQ score of 128,
that's relatively consistent.
-
I think that's what you meant
-
Alicia when you said accurate,
-
but it's about consistency,
-
so having almost the same score.
-
I would have low
test-retest reliability if
-
one day I score a
40 on an IQ test,
-
and then the next
time I score 140,
-
so that doesn't have good
test-retest reliability.
-
Perfect. We talked about
inter-rater reliability.
-
Does anyone want to
take a stab at that?
-
Maybe Meghan? Or Rick.
-
Well, do the same test,
-
but it's not you doing it again.
-
It's two different people
-
doing the same test on the same.
-
That's what it is. You're
looking for consistent scores,
-
but using two different
people doing this film.
-
Yes, can you give us an example?
-
I came up with really
silly examples,
-
so just go for it.
-
I test to see if,
-
I run around the block
and I get sweaty,
-
and then someone else
-
test to see if running
around the block.
-
No, that's wrong. I don't
know. I can't think.
-
No, that's okay. I think
-
the key here is that
there are two observers.
-
There's two observers.
-
Yes.
-
But for the same testing.
-
Yes. In your example,
-
Alicia would be my
research assistant,
-
and she would be coding for how
-
sweaty you get
versus someone else.
-
I would also have Hailey.
-
Inter-rater reliability
means that Alicia
-
and Hailey score you similarly
on how sweaty you get,
-
and Hailey and Alicia score
-
the other person similarly
in how sweaty they get.
-
It's two observers looking at
-
behavior in someone else.
Does that make sense?
-
Yeah, thank you.
-
Yeah, I think you
were almost there.
-
You just had the
two people down,
-
but not sure where it
went, but that's good.
-
That's still good. What
about internal reliability?
-
Isn't this where if R
is positive and strong,
-
you have a good reliability?
-
Yes, but what does that mean?
-
I'm not quite positive.
-
Who was that?
-
Grace.
-
Grace. Yeah. Good
job, Grace. Hailey.
-
Isn't it where, when you ask
questions to the people,
-
so it's like two
different questions,
-
but they both are trying
to get at the same answer.
-
They're just worded differently,
-
and the people answer
them consistently.
-
Yes. Rick.
-
Same question, word
it differently.
-
Yes. In Hailey's
questionnaire that I created,
-
it would be if you ask people,
-
how likely are you or how
-
often do you daydream about
burning your office down?
-
Then also, if your
office was burning down,
-
would you call the
fire department?
-
Those are questions that
are framed differently,
-
but also measuring the same,
-
like, how much do you
-
appreciate or want
to work at your job?
-
Oh, okay.
-
Yeah. They would
answer consistently.
-
Good. I'm going to keep
-
asking these questions
just to make sure.
-
It's okay if you don't
get the entire measure
-
or the type of
reliability, 100% there.
-
As you can tell, most of you
were 80% there. It's okay.
-
And during the exam,
it will be open book,
-
open note at the
end of the term,
-
which is also, I guess,
the end of the year.
-
Still good job. We
talked about this.
-
We looked at the measurement
of head circumference,
-
at Time 1 and Time 2.
-
Which reliability would this be?
-
You measure the head at Time 1,
-
you measure the head at Time 2,
-
and the measures are pretty
related to one another.
-
Which of the reliabilities
would this be?
-
Test-retest.
-
Yeah. Perfect. This is
inter-rater reliability.
-
Does anyone want to
explain the difference
-
between the left and
the right to me?
-
The left one is showing
that both observers,
-
their ratings are very
close to one another,
-
whereas the right
side is showing
-
they're all over the place and
-
not quite matching
up with one another.
-
Perfect. You would
say that this one has
-
high interrater reliability and
-
this one has low
interrater reliability
-
because we have two observers,
-
so Mark's ratings
and Matt's ratings,
-
and they both seem to
track on the same line.
-
But if we have Mark's
ratings and Peter's ratings,
-
they don't seem to track on
-
the same line;
they're scattered.
-
This would mean that this has
low interrater reliability,
-
so I might not trust
Peter's ratings.
-
Maybe he's using a
different assessment
-
for sweatiness if we're testing
-
Rick and other people's
sweatiness when they run,
-
but Mark and Matt seem to
-
be on point with what
they're measuring.
-
I believe this is where we were,
-
and this is what Grace
was talking about.
-
Using the correlation
coefficient
-
r to evaluate reliability.
-
The correlation
coefficient finds
-
the relationship between
two different variables.
-
Someone give me two
variables that could be
-
related or maybe not related.
-
Foot size and height.
-
Foot size and height.
We have foot size
-
being X and height being Y.
-
We could use all
of these different
-
scatter plots to represent that.
-
Another thing to note for
the correlation coefficient,
-
the values go from
negative one to one,
-
so it includes zero.
-
The sign in front of the number,
-
whether it's negative
or positive,
-
tells you the slope direction.
-
If the slope seems to be
increasing like this one,
-
all the data seem
to be increasing,
-
as X increases, Y increases;
-
you would say that this
is a positive slope.
-
As X increases as shoe size
increases, height increases.
-
This would also be
a positive slope.
-
This one, on the other hand,
-
would be a negative
slope because as
-
shoe size increases,
height decreases.
-
If you're a size, let's
just say this is 11,
-
then you are five foot or
four foot two or something.
-
as shoe size increases,
height decreases.
-
But this one doesn't really
have a slope because there's
-
no way to predict as X
increases what Y would be,
-
since it's all scattered around.
-
You can see what the
slope direction is or
-
whether there's a positive
value or negative value
-
by looking at the slope of
the pattern of the dots.
-
Does that make
sense? Then you can
-
tell the strength by how
closely together the dots are.
-
I would imagine a line going
straight diagonally here,
-
and how well do the dots
match up to that line?
-
They don't match up
super well here.
-
This is a 0.56 relationship.
-
Again, from negative
one to one, this one,
-
if you had a line,
-
they all seem to
track really well.
-
This is an r of 0.93.
-
You can tell that it's
really strong by how well
-
the dots adhere to one another.
-
This one is -0.59,
-
so it's a little better
than this one or a
-
little stronger
than this example.
-
This r of 0.01,
-
there's no strength, it's
not really predictive.
-
Perfect. Let me see
-
my notes if I need to
tell you anything else.
-
Any questions here?
-
Which is the strongest r
in these scatter plots?
-
R 0.93.
-
Perfect. If it was a -0.93,
-
would it still be the strongest?
-
Or actually, negative 0.94.
-
I think so because
-
negative correlations
can also be strong.
-
It just depends on
what you're measuring.
-
Perfect. It doesn't really
-
matter whether it has a
positive or negative.
-
It's more about the absolute
value of the r. Of these,
-
the r 0.93 is the strongest,
-
and which one is the weakest,
-
Rick or Elysia, or Grace?
-
I can't see the numbers,
-
but the scattering ones.
-
This one.
-
That one. 0.01, yeah.
-
Perfect. How does
the correlation
-
coefficient evaluate reliability
-
for the test-retest reliability?
-
You would correlate
their first IQ score to
-
their second IQ score and
see how strong it is.
-
If your first IQ score was
-
125 and your second
IQ score was 126,
-
they would correlate
pretty highly.
-
Your r would be very positive.
-
If r is strong and positive,
-
at least 0.05 or above,
you have good test.
-
I think it should
be 0.50 or above.
-
Then you have good
test-retest reliability.
-
If r is positive at weak,
-
this is a sign of low
test-retest reliability.
-
This reliability is
only relevant for
-
variables that don't
change over time.
-
It wouldn't be relevant
for something like weight.
-
Weight tends to fluctuate
more than shoe size, maybe.
-
What about for
interrater reliability?
-
Two observers rate the same
-
participants at the same time,
-
and then r is computed.
-
You want to see how Hailey and
-
Elysia's evaluation of
sweating after running,
-
how related they
are to one another.
-
Here, if r is
positive and strong,
-
the value has to
be 0.7 or higher;
-
you have good
interrater reliability.
-
Here, you want it to be
positive, not negative.
-
If r is positive but weak,
-
you don't have good
interrater reliability.
-
Negative r in this situation
would be meaningful,
-
and it would mean terrible
interrater reliability.
-
If you had a -0.7,
-
that would mean that when
Hailey rates the person
-
as 5 on sweatiness,
-
Elysia would rate them
as -5 on sweatiness.
-
As Hailey's scores go up,
-
Elysia's scores go down,
-
or the other way around.
-
Internal reliability, this is
-
when you have more than one item
-
to tap into the construct.
-
This is like the example
that I gave with
-
Hailey's burning
the building down.
-
I would correlate the two values
-
to see how related they
are to one another.
-
Another important
thing to note is,
-
I asked one question
where positive means more
-
likely to burn the
employment place down.
-
The other question, I said,
-
how likely are you to
-
prevent the burning
of the place down?
-
I need to reverse
code that item,
-
and we'll talk about
what that means,
-
and then correlate the items to
-
see how well that taps
into the construct
-
of workplace satisfaction.
Does that make sense?
-
I must hear more about
reverse code. I like that.
-
We'll talk about that, and
I'll show you how to do that,
-
but maybe not in this lecture.
-
Essentially, what
you want to do with
-
reverse coding is you want to
-
make sure that all of your
items mean the same thing.
-
I want to tap into
the construct of
-
workplace satisfaction
by assessing
-
desire to burn your
workplace down.
-
The question, how
likely are you to
-
call the fire department
-
if your workplace
was burning down,
-
is not necessarily measuring
your workplace satisfaction,
-
but it might be
measuring how likely you
-
are to not want it to burn down.
-
But if I want to measure
-
how much I want the
building to burn down,
-
then I need to
reverse code that.
-
If you said, strongly disagree,
-
I would not want to
call a fire department;
-
that would mean now
with reverse coding,
-
it would be strongly agree.
-
I would word the
question instead
-
as I would not be likely to
call the fire department,
-
and then strongly agree.
-
But it'll make more sense once
-
we get to that part
of the chapter.
-
Does it make sense now?
-
Yeah.
-
That is almost word for word
what I said. Yes, Hailey.
-
Just like what you were talking
about the reverse coding,
-
I've taken personality
tests for previous classes.
-
Is that why some questions
are just the same words,
-
but worded backwards on the
big five personality tests?
-
Exactly. That's why they're
-
worded backwards because
we want to make sure
-
that you answer consistently
irrespective of
-
how we word the questions
because if I asked you,
-
on a scale of 1-7, how
happy are you from one,
-
no emotions, to seven,
extremely ecstatic.
-
I might also ask you,
-
how unhappy are you from one,
-
to not at all,
-
to seven, no emotions.
-
Then I'd reverse code one of
-
the items to actually
assess happiness.
-
I've always seen those
questions and I'm like,
-
didn't I just answer
questions exactly like
-
this, but that makes sense.
-
Rick. Sorry.
-
Hey, [inaudible] exactly
what you're saying.
-
That's exactly what I'm
thinking when I'm reading this.
-
It just clarified something.
-
It was really good
because all the
-
time in all these surveys,
-
I'm going, I've just read
that question, so this helps.
-
That's the reason why we do it.
-
We want to make sure that
there's reliability in
-
the scale because if you
-
answer the questions
differently,
-
then that means that
we're not appropriately
-
measuring the construct or we're
-
not measuring it as well
as we thought we would.
-
I said those in words so
you can have that later.
-
What about in journal articles?
-
Rick, you'll probably
read about this.
-
All of you will probably
read about this, but Rick,
-
you were talking
about physiology,
-
and self-report, and
things like that.
-
You'll see the reliability
presented like this.
-
I'm trying to get
my mouse over here.
-
Test-retest reliability,
for instance, 0.83,
-
0.84, 0.6, 0.82, 0.5, 0.54.
-
Hailey, are you looking
at a meta analysis?
-
You might find this too.
-
I have, I think,
-
two articles that
are meta analysis.
-
You might find some values
like this for test-retest.
-
This is telling you how
long between the tests.
-
This is a satisfaction with
-
life scale, but
they're different.
-
There's Alfonso and Allison,
-
Pavot et al, Blais et al,
-
Diener et al, and
so on and so forth.
-
If you take the tests
that Alfonso and Allison
-
gave at Time 1 and
two weeks later,
-
the test-retest reliability
is 0.83 with one month.
-
With a Pavot et al, 0.84.
-
Two months, you see
it going down, 0.64.
-
The Diener et al
test-retest reliability
-
is actually higher
at two months.
-
At 10 weeks, of course,
-
it goes down to 0.5,
-
and in four years,
-
it goes down to 0.54.
-
That means that the
scores that you have at
-
Time 1 aren't necessarily as
-
related at Time 2.
Does that make sense?
-
The coefficient Alpha is for
the internal reliability.
-
How good do all of the questions
-
that are in that scale measure
-
the construct that
you're interested in?
-
Here you want a good value.
-
How well does, I'm very happy
-
and I feel really sad,
reverse-coded measure happiness?
-
Here, the coefficient
Alpha is 0.89.
-
That's good because
it's close to one.
-
0.85, still good,
0.79-0.84 still good.
-
All of these are
greater than 0.70,
-
which means that all the items
-
tend to hang together nicely.
-
Does that make sense?
-
That's what you'll see in
-
the text or any
articles that you read.
-
Now we're going to talk about
validity of measurement.
-
We're going to talk about
measurement validity
-
of abstract constructs.
-
We've talked about reliability.
-
Now we're moving on to validity.
-
What is the difference between
-
face validity and
content validity?
-
Does it look like
a good measure?
-
Criterion validity.
-
Does it correlate
with key behaviors.
-
Convergent validity and
discriminate validity.
-
Does a pattern make sense?
-
Relationship between
reliability and validity.
-
What are the three
reliabilities again?
-
Rick. Alesia or whoever
was saying that.
-
Test-retest. I keep
wanting to say relator.
-
Interrater.
-
Interrater. My gosh. I'm
blanking on the third one.
-
Internal. How do we
measure the strength of
-
those reliabilities?
It's a letter.
-
The coefficient R.
-
Coefficient R. The
coefficient R,
-
what are the endpoints?
Or what's the range?
-
Negative one to one
including [inaudible]
-
Perfect. What do
we want the value
-
to be to be good reliability?
-
We want it to be positive and
closer to one as possible.
-
That's what I've been lecturing
about for the last hour.
-
[LAUGHTER] Also last
lecture and this lecture.
-
I just want to make sure we
are all on the same page.
-
Now we're moving to validity.
-
Let's move on to validity.
-
We talked about
reliability here,
-
so test-retest, internal,
and interrater reliability.
-
Now, we're going to move on to
-
here with construct validity,
-
and we'll talk about all of
these different validities,
-
but we're mostly
going to be talking
-
about construct validity here.
-
We're going to jump into
-
two subjective ways
to assess validity,
-
so face validity and
content validity.
-
Face validity is exactly
what it sounds like.
-
Does your measure look like
what you want to measure?
-
Then content validity
is the measure
-
contains all the parts that
-
your theory says
it should contain.
-
We have different standards for
-
measurements as
physical scientists,
-
so we can use rulers, scales,
-
thermometers to ensure that
-
we're measuring
things like length,
-
weight, and
temperature reliably.
-
However, psychological
scientists
-
are interested in measuring
-
abstract concepts
like self-esteem,
-
or how much you hate
your job, or depression.
-
That's more abstract.
-
Because of that, our
construct validity is a lot
-
more important to
psychological scientists.
-
For people who measure
physical things like
-
weight, well,
construct validity.
-
How would you measure weight?
-
The scale.
-
That's pretty easy, but for
more abstract concepts,
-
construct validity
is super important.
-
Let's jump into face validity
and content validity.
-
Like I said, face
validity is it looks
-
like what you want to measure
and content validity,
-
the measure contains all of
-
the parts your theory
says it should contain.
-
Alesia, what is
your study about?
-
How introversion and
-
extroversion affect
anxiety management.
-
Having good validity would
-
be asking questions
related to introversion.
-
Do you enjoy spending
time with others?
-
Do you get energized by spending
-
time with others?
Things like that.
-
It seems to have good face
validity and content validity.
-
Contains all the parts that
-
your theory says
it should contain.
-
With introversion, we're talking
-
about how much time you
like spending with others,
-
so we're asking those questions,
-
but if I ask
questions related to,
-
how much time you spend online,
-
that might not be good content
validity because it's not
-
assessing how much time you
enjoy spending with others.
-
[inaudible]
-
It could if you did how
much time you spent on
-
Facebook talking to people
or whatever the case may be.
-
Does that make sense, face
-
validity and content validity?
-
If I was measuring happiness,
-
it might be asking questions
about how happy you are,
-
how sad you are.
-
It might not have as
-
good content validity if
I ask how angry you are,
-
how violent you are.
-
That might not be a great
measure of happiness.
-
It might be a great
measure of likelihood to
-
engage in violence,
but not happiness.
-
Then we have criterion validity,
-
and this is when you
want to see whether
-
your variable of interest
correlates with key behaviors.
-
Most psychological
scientists prefer to rely on
-
empirical assessments of
-
validity over
subjective judgments.
-
One way to empirically
assess validity is
-
by examining criterion
validity, which is here.
-
Whether the measure
is related to
-
a concrete outcome that
it should be related to.
-
Let's say you work
for a company that
-
wants to predict how well
-
job applicants do a salespeople.
-
Initially, the company used
-
IQ scores to predict sales
aptitude. Here we go.
-
The criterion
validity. Right here,
-
we're checking how
well aptitude test
-
A relates to sale figures
in thousands of dollars.
-
You wanted your IQ test to
-
predict or relate to a
behavior, sales figures.
-
As you can tell,
-
as aptitude test scores go up,
-
so do sales figures in
dollars. They're, great.
-
We can use IQ to predict how
well our employees will do.
-
Let's say that then they
also have aptitude test B,
-
which is a different IQ test.
-
They're looking at
how that relates to
-
sales figures and what they
-
find because these dots are a
-
lot more scattered
from that line,
-
that aptitude test B
doesn't have as great of
-
a criterion validity
as aptitude test A.
-
That means that aptitude
test B is less likely to
-
predict sales figures
relative to aptitude test A.
-
Is that clear? Just
criterion validity is you're
-
using one measure to
-
predict behaviors or
something another scale.
-
You're comparing the measures
that you're going to
-
use to see which one
is best serious.
-
No, that's good.
-
For the burning of the building,
-
you might want to use burning
of the building scale
-
to what people actually do
when the building is on fire,
-
how likely you are
to put the fire
-
out if there is an actual fire.
-
What you find is maybe
-
people burning saying
that they would burn
-
a building is not the same
as what they actually do in
-
a fire so might not have
great criterion validity.
-
Another way to gather evidence
for criterion validity
-
is to use a known
groups paradigm.
-
What is a knowns group paradigm?
-
This is when you have
-
a group that you know
that scores high
-
on this measure or on
a related measure,
-
and you create a new measure.
-
For instance, the Beck
Depression Inventory
-
is one of the most
popularly used inventory
-
to measure depression.
-
But when it first came out,
-
there were already existing
measures of depression.
-
If you want to create
a new measure,
-
you have to have a good reason
to include that measure.
-
The BEC depression inventory is
-
a 21 item, self report scales.
-
In order to test the
criterion validity
-
of this depression scale,
-
you could administer the BDI
-
to a group of people
with depression,
-
so are already in treatment for
-
depression and a group of
people who aren't depressed.
-
If you find here,
-
this is the known group.
-
You have a group of
people who are depressed.
-
You have an inventory that
should measure depression.
-
If you find that depressed
people score higher on
-
your depression
inventory relative
-
to those that are not depressed,
-
then that tells us that there's
good criterion validity.
-
The depression
inventory actually
-
predicts who is depressed
versus who is not.
-
This is the known groups method.
-
Another way is to separate
-
the depression that people
are experiencing from mild,
-
moderate, and severe.
-
This could be the
psychiatrists rating of how
-
much their patients are
experiencing depression,
-
and the BDI score,
-
the BEC depression
inventory should be able
-
to distinguish between
those who have mild,
-
moderate and severe depression,
which happens here.
-
Severe people are experiencing
the most amount of
-
BDI relative to those who are
-
moderate and mild depression.
-
Is that clear? Do you guys
-
like me to use the
weird examples.
-
Example. We're good.
-
I was fine. The burning
-
the house down or burning
your place of employment,
-
you might have pyromaniacs and
-
measure whether
pyromaniacs would
-
score higher on the
likelihood to burn
-
the employment down relative to
-
those who are not pyromaniacs,
-
and you would want
people who are
-
pyromaniacs to score higher on
-
likelihood of burning place
of employment down relative
-
to those that
aren't pyromaniacs.
-
That would be a good
measure of fire.
-
I actually like my job.
-
I don't know why I came
up with that. [LAUGHTER]
-
It's very dark.
-
It is very dark. I don't
-
know I came up
with that example.
-
Perfect. Does that make sense?
-
Your measure should be able to
-
predict likelihood of starting
-
a fire and the degree
-
to which you would be
likely to start a fire.
-
Pyromaniac should
be high here and
-
then mild pyromaniacs versus
-
moderate pyromaniacs
versus severe pyromaniacs,
-
it should also be
able to predict
-
the extent to which
people are pyromaniacs.
-
That's one way to establish
-
whether your scale
is good or not.
-
We can also use known
groups evidence
-
for criterion validity.
-
This is Diener's subjective
well being scale.
-
Remember, Diener created
this to measure happiness.
-
We're using it for different
groups of individuals.
-
What we are using is
American college students,
-
French Canadian
college students,
-
Korean university students,
printing trade workers,
-
veterans Affairs
hospital patients,
-
abused women, and
male prison innates.
-
These are the references where
they got the scores from.
-
What the Diener's subjective
well being scale would want
-
is scores that map on to
-
these different
measures of happiness.
-
Does that make sense? When
you measure happiness
-
with Pavot and Diener measure,
-
you have a 6.4 of
-
happiness for American
college students.
-
With Blais et al, you have 6.1,
-
for Suh study, it's 5.8,
-
for George, it's 6.0,
-
for veterans Affairs
hospital inpatients
-
with Frisch, it is 5.6,
-
for Fisher at 7.4,
-
and that's the
standard deviation.
-
Abused women is 20.7,
-
and male prison inmates
with Joy is 12.3.
-
Just by looking at this,
-
do you think that
-
these measures are okay
measures of happiness?
-
For instance, who is the
happiest based on these values?
-
Printing trade workers.
-
Printing trade workers,
-
their jobs are
probably pretty chill.
-
American college students or
-
these college students might
still be super stressed out,
-
but they're still
relatively happy.
-
What about male prison inmates
-
and Veterans Affair
hospital inpatients?
-
They're pretty low on happiness.
-
It tracks so it does seem
-
to accurately measure happiness.
-
Because the actual values
-
of happiness are tracking
what we would expect,
-
then we can say that the
subjective well-being scale
-
would have high
criterion validity.
-
It actually measures
what it's intended to
-
measure. Does that make sense?
-
Yeah, I can read that one.
-
This one's very clear. Now we
-
have convergent validity
and discriminant validity.
-
This is concerned
with whether there is
-
a meaningful pattern of
similarities and differences.
-
Convergent, as you might guess,
-
is how well does the BDI,
-
Beck Depression
Inventory relate to
-
an already existing
inventory for depression,
-
which is the Center
for Epidemiologic
-
Studies Depression Scale.
-
It's not hard work.
-
The Center for Epidemiologic,
-
no I can't say it.
-
You know what I'm trying to say.
-
Studies Depression Scale,
-
and these have a correlation
coefficient of 0.68.
-
I'm just going to
call it the CES-D.
-
When the Beck Depression
Inventory came on the scene,
-
this was the most popularly
used measure of depression.
-
But the BDI needs to
-
be similar or converge
-
on an already existing
measure of depression,
-
but it also needs to be slightly
-
unique because if it's
exactly the same,
-
then why are we
creating a new scale?
-
Right here, let's say
-
we have people that we
know are depressed,
-
and they take both
CES-D and the BDI.
-
What we see is that the
scores do tend to converge.
-
As people increase in BDI,
-
they also tend to
increase in CES-D.
-
But it's not a perfect
relationship, and that's fine.
-
We don't actually want
it to be perfect.
-
Now, discriminant
validity is how
-
well and how different is this
scale from other measures.
-
A measure should
correlate less strongly
-
with measures of
different constructs.
-
In other words, there
must be differences.
-
The Beck Depression Inventory
is supposed to measure
-
psychological well being or
-
non-well being and not
-
necessarily physical
health problems.
-
If this has good
discriminate validity,
-
which it does, that
means that your BDI,
-
Beck Depression
Inventory should be not
-
related to physical
health problems
-
and that's what we find.
-
As BDI increases, it doesn't
-
necessarily predict how many
health problems you'll have.
-
Convergent validity
is how well does
-
your measure map onto
already existing measures,
-
and discriminant
validity is about how
-
much your measure correlates
-
with measures of
different constructs.
-
Think of convergent as same and
-
discriminant as different
with different scales.
-
Any questions here?
Then the relationship
-
between reliability
and validity.
-
We talked a lot about
reliability and validity.
-
Remember that just
because a measure is
-
reliable doesn't mean
it's a valid measure.
-
For instance, I could measure
my cat's head every day,
-
and I'll have a really reliable
measure of my cat's head,
-
but that might not be
a valid measure of IQ.
-
Head size might not be
a valid measure of IQ,
-
meaning that it doesn't
actually measure IQ.
-
It's reliable, but
it's not valid.
-
Reliability is more of how well
-
a measure correlates
with itself.
-
Invalidity is how
well a measure is
-
associated with something else.
-
A measure can be less
valid than it is reliable,
-
but it cannot be more
valid than it is reliable.
-
For instance, a measure can
be less valid than it is
-
reliable so measuring
my cat's head
-
might not be related to its IQ,
-
so it's less valid
than it's reliable,
-
but it can't be more
valid than it's reliable.
-
IQ scores cannot be a
more valid measure of
-
head circumference relative to
-
how reliable the head
circumference and IQ is.
-
Reliability is necessary but
not sufficient for validity.
-
These are just things
to consider as you're
-
thinking about your projects.
-
There will be some checks and
-
balances that you'll have
to go through when you're
-
thinking about validity
and reliability.
-
You'll want your scales to be
-
reliable and you'll also
want them to be valid.
-
Reliable means that the scales
-
actually measure what you
want them to measure,
-
and validity is how well
-
that measure is associated
with something else.
-
For instance, Hailey, if you're
-
interested in job persistence,
-
you want the measure of job
persistence to actually
-
predict the people being
-
in their jobs maybe
five years later.
-
That would be the validity.
-
That is it for today.
-
Any questions?
-
We're going through
a lot of chapters,
-
and I think it's good
that we're actually
-
keeping on track.
-
But if you don't have any more
questions, then that's it.
-
Enjoy your weekend.
Alasia, if you
-
want to stay. Thank you.
-
Thank you.
-
Thanks.
-
Thank you.
-
Thank you.
-
Let's see. I stopped
recording. I think I did.