-
We talked about
classical conditioning,
-
but of course, there are some
-
limits to classical
conditioning.
-
You can't classically
condition all behaviors.
-
One, classical
conditioning can't
-
teach new behaviors
to an organism,
-
but it can only elicit innate
or otherwise reflexive
-
behaviors with stimuli that
-
ordinarily might not
have these effects.
-
For example, I gave
-
an example where my cat
-
came running to
the sound of food.
-
Seeking food is an
innate behavior
-
that exists within cats.
-
The only thing are
all creatures.
-
The only association
that I created
-
was the sound of a can opening
-
with this already
innate behavior.
-
I couldn't have
taught her to run
-
towards the sound of
-
a bicycle because cats don't
-
necessarily have that
ability innately built in.
-
Another element of
classical conditioning
-
can be outlined by these
three important factors.
-
The important factors
must be met in order
-
for classical conditioning
to actually work.
-
First is cognition.
-
The second factor is
-
the evolutionary history
of the organism,
-
and third is the ecology
of the organism.
-
We'll go through each of
-
these individually on
the next few slides.
-
First, there's cognition
and evolution.
-
Robert Rescorla
demonstrated that
-
conditioning rats to fear
-
a tone stimulus didn't
-
work unless the tone reliably
predicted the shocks,
-
so the conditioned stimulus.
-
Rescorla proposed that
the rats were engaging in
-
cognitive activity to determine
-
whether the stimulus
was a valid predictor.
-
All of the creatures that
we classically condition,
-
whether it's dogs, cats, rats,
-
or humans, they
engage in cognition
-
in order for classical
conditioning to work.
-
In this experiment, the
rats only became afraid of
-
the tone if the tone reliably
predicted the shocks.
-
Here, the rats are conditioned
-
to fear response when the tone
-
is sounded because every
-
time there's a tone,
there's a shock.
-
For these rats in Group
2, the tone appears,
-
they get shocked, but then
-
they get shocked even when
the tone isn't there,
-
the tone appears,
they get shocked.
-
Rats aren't conditioned to
fear response when the tone is
-
sounded because the tone
-
isn't always paired
with a shock.
-
Thus, the rats are engaging
in some cognitive activity to
-
determine that the
tone is either
-
predictive or not
predictive of a shock.
-
When it comes to
cognition and evolution,
-
John Garcia overturned
the general
-
process behaviorist doctrine
of equipotentiality,
-
which proposed that virtually
any neutral stimulus could
-
be used to condition
virtually any response.
-
Garcia's research showed that
-
the evolutionary history of
each organism prepares it
-
to be more or less easily
conditioned by various stimuli.
-
In the textbook, you'll
read an instance
-
where either noises
were presented
-
alongside the ingestion of
-
sugar water that
made the mice sick,
-
feel nauseous, or
radiation was presented
-
along with the ingestion of
-
this sugar water that made
the mice feel nauseous.
-
For the mice, the
noises were more
-
effective than radiation
to help the mice
-
avoid the sugar water because
the noises were something
-
that they could
interpret and perceive,
-
whereas the radiation wasn't
-
necessarily something
that they could perceive.
-
He showed that not
necessarily any stimulus
-
can be used to condition
virtually any response.
-
The creature in question
has to have the ability to
-
interpret that
unconditioned stimulus.
-
The evolutionary history of
-
each organism prepares
it to be more or
-
less easily conditioned
by various stimuli.
-
Then we also have the
functionalist perspective
-
that notes some limitations
of classical conditioning.
-
The functionalist
perspective proposes
-
that classical
conditioning occurs in
-
natural environments as
well as in the laboratory.
-
Classical conditioning
in the wild
-
likely evolved to help
an organism survive,
-
whereas the motive behind
-
conditioning might be
different in the lab.
-
Conditioning stimuli
and the natural ecology
-
of an animal are rarely
neutral or arbitrary.
-
They actually do have
some predictive validity.
-
It's not just random,
whereas in the lab,
-
the neutral stimuli
might be something that
-
isn't available in the
creature's normal environment.
-
Conditioning that
occurs in nature
-
may be more rapid, robust,
-
and less dependent upon a
very small time interval
-
between the presentation of
-
the conditioning stimulus and
-
the unconditioned stimulus,
-
and thus when the pairing of
a conditioned stimulus with
-
the unconditioned stimulus is
-
presented in the animal's
natural environment,
-
it's more resistant to
-
interference in the
conditioning process
-
relative to those
that are paired
-
or the associations that
are made in the laboratory.
-
Those are the limitations.
-
We're talking about context.
-
We talked about
evolutionary mechanisms
-
that make certain stimuli more
-
likely to be successfully
conditioned to a response.
-
We also talked about how
-
cognition plays a role in
classical conditioning.
-
Now let's talk about
operant conditioning,
-
which is also a different
learning that occurs.
-
Operant conditioning
was developed as
-
a response or as
-
an extension of
Thorndike's Law of Effect.
-
Now, in Thorndike's
Law of Effect,
-
he states that for any organism,
-
those behaviors that lead to
-
a satisfying state of
affairs are stamped
-
in while those
behaviors that lead to
-
an unsatisfying
or annoying state
-
of affairs are stamped out.
-
In other words, learning among
-
organisms doesn't
necessarily only
-
occur through classical
conditioning,
-
but it occurs through
the gain of resources
-
or something that the creature
-
really seeks and if
-
any behavior leads to
negative outcomes,
-
and those behaviors
are stamped out.
-
In other words, the organism
learns as a result of
-
the consequences of his
or her own actions.
-
This is Thorndike's
Law of Effect,
-
which led to the development
of operant conditioning.
-
This is a type of learning in
-
which the consequences
of a behavior
-
affect the probability
that the behavior
-
will be repeated in the future.
-
In this instance,
-
in this example that
you'll see here,
-
the organism operates on
-
his or her environment
in a way that results in
-
specific consequences
and comes to
-
associate these consequences
with its behavior.
-
Operant conditioning
is an active process
-
entirely shaped by the
organism's own behavior.
-
Whereas when we were
-
talking about classical
conditioning,
-
that tends to be more of
-
a process that occurs
below conscious awareness,
-
whereas operant
conditioning occurs
-
and is an active process.
-
Here we see a rat and there's
-
a little food tray that's
connected to food pellets,
-
and there is a little light,
-
and the little rat
can press a lever.
-
The rat engages with the lever
-
and based on whether
the light is on or not,
-
and the rat's own behavior,
-
the rat learns that maybe
-
if the light is on
and I push the lever,
-
food will come out.
-
The rat learns that it is up to
-
his or her own
behavior that leads
-
to food being delivered
to his food tray.
-
How does operant conditioning
-
differ from classical
conditioning?
-
I already gave you one example.
-
Operant conditioning
is more active,
-
requires more active engagement
-
from the creature relative
to classical conditioning.
-
Again, operant conditioning is
-
an active process that is shaped
-
by an organism's own behavior,
-
whereas classical conditioning
is a passive process
-
shaped by forces outside the
control of the organism.
-
For example, with Pavlov's dogs,
-
the dogs couldn't ring the
bell and then salivate.
-
It was up to Pavlov
to ring the bell and
-
then it would cause
the dogs to salivate.
-
Even salivation
was a process that
-
occurred below the dog's
conscious awareness.
-
They didn't have the ability
to control their salivation.
-
Operant conditioning,
like I said,
-
it functions as a result of
-
an organism's own behavior
-
and their behavior either
-
leads to positive reinforcement,
-
negative reinforcement,
positive punishment,
-
and negative punishment.
-
Now, these terms
might be a little
-
confusing because when we
think of reinforcement,
-
we think of something
that might be positive,
-
something that
might be a reward,
-
but that's not what we're
talking about in this instance.
-
Also, when we think
about punishment,
-
we might think of something
that is punitive,
-
something that is negative,
-
something that is meant to
discourage a certain behavior.
-
Let's talk about what
each of these mean.
-
When we talk about
reinforcement here,
-
what we mean is that reinforcers
increase the behavior,
-
and positive means you add
-
something to increase
the behavior.
-
Negative means you remove
-
something to increase
the behavior.
-
When it comes to punishment,
-
punishers decrease a behavior,
-
so you add something to
-
decrease the behavior
with positive punishment,
-
or you remove something
to decrease the behavior.
-
With positive reinforcement,
-
when the frequency or
probability of the behavior
-
is increased as a result
-
of the addition or
presentation of
-
something following the
performance of a behavior.
-
In this instance, it
would be like the rat
-
getting food pellets when
-
the light is on and
it presses the lever.
-
The frequency or
probability of behavior is
-
increased as a result of
-
the addition or presentation of
-
something following
the performance
-
of the behavior, the pellets.
-
Negative reinforcement when
a behavior is reinforced
-
by the removal or
avoidance of something.
-
Positive punishment is when
-
the consequence of a behavior is
-
the addition or presentation of
-
something, generally
something aversive.
-
Let's say if the rat
-
presses the lever when
the light isn't on,
-
they receive a shock.
-
That would be
positive punishment.
-
Negative punishment is
when the consequence of
-
a behavior is the
removal of something.
-
Let's go through and look at
-
this table that I created to
-
help you all understand what
positive reinforcement,
-
negative reinforcement
and positive punishment,
-
negative punishment look like.
-
Students frequently
have a hard time
-
distinguishing the implications
of positive and negative.
-
The important thing to
emphasize is that reinforcers
-
increase the behavior and
-
punishers decrease the behavior.
-
I put that here,
you might want to
-
remember that for the exam.
-
For positive reinforcement, you
-
add something to
increase the behavior.
-
A child puts toys away
and receives a cookie,
-
the child learns to
put their toys away.
-
For negative punishment,
-
you take away something
-
to decrease a behavior.
-
This would be if a child
is mean to a sibling,
-
you send him to his room
and he learns to be nicer.
-
For positive punishment, you add
-
something to
decrease a behavior.
-
You're late to a
meeting and your boss
-
criticizes you in
front of everyone.
-
You learn to avoid being late.
-
For negative reinforcement,
you take away
-
something that increases
your behavior.
-
You run into the
house to get out of
-
a storm and you learn
to avoid the rain.
-
Another way to think about
this if you're having
-
trouble during the exam is
-
to think of this positive
and negative reinforcement
-
and punishment in the
context of substance abuse.
-
For positive reinforcement,
you might get social gain,
-
physiological responses
to the drug or the high.
-
When you're taking drugs,
-
you might become more popular
at the moment or you might
-
find a new group of
-
friends, that's
positive reinforcement.
-
You add something to
increase the behavior.
-
With negative
reinforcement, that
-
would be decreasing
your anxiety or
-
depression through self
medicating with a drug and
-
it might alleviate
social pressure
-
to actually engage
in taking the drug.
-
For positive punishment,
-
you add something to
decrease the behavior.
-
You might have health concerns,
-
so you might have dependency,
-
cirrhosis of the
liver or cancer,
-
you might also experience
-
legal troubles as a result of
taking the drugs so that'll
-
decrease your behavior of
-
taking drugs and
negative punishment,
-
you take away something
to decrease the behavior,
-
so you go to jail,
-
which takes away your freedom
-
or you lose your employment.
-
Another factor that contributes
-
to operant conditioning is
-
whether the reinforcement is
continuous versus partial.
-
Do you always receive
this reinforcement
-
or is it only
partially reinforced?
-
Partial reinforcement
is when a behavior is
-
reinforced periodically
rather than continuously.
-
Although behaviors
may be acquired very
-
quickly using continuous
reinforcement schedules,
-
they are generally more
enduring and difficult to
-
extinguish when they are
-
conditioned with
partial reinforcement.
-
The partial reinforcement is
-
called the partial
reinforcement effect.
-
There are different reinforcement
schedules that one may
-
follow in order to elicit
different behaviors.
-
One, you have a fixed
interval schedule.
-
This is when you provide
reinforcement for
-
the first response after
-
a specific interval
of time has passed.
-
For example, after a minute,
-
after a few hours,
-
after a few days,
-
the number of responses
really has no effect
-
on the timing of
the reinforcement.
-
You also have variable
interval schedules,
-
and this is where you
provide reinforcement
-
at unpredictable time intervals.
-
Every five minutes on average,
-
for example, you could also
have a fixed ratio schedule.
-
This provides
reinforcement after
-
a specific number of responses.
-
Let's say every five times you
-
hit the lever to
get food pellets.
-
Then you have variable ratio.
-
This provides
reinforcement after
-
an unpredictable
number of responses.
-
We talked about the limits
of classical conditioning.
-
Now let's talk about the limits
of operant conditioning.
-
One, we have Tolman's concept
-
of the cognitive map
latent learning.
-
In this concept,
-
they were the first
substantial challenge
-
to the view that all behavior
-
could be explained
through contingencies
-
of reinforcement and punishment.
-
What this means is that
-
without reinforcement
or punishment,
-
learning still takes place,
-
but it might not be
apparent in behavior.
-
Think about you
learning in this class,
-
it might not always take
punishment or reinforcement.
-
You might still learn.
-
The evolutionary
history of an organism
-
puts constraints on what it
-
may learn and how easily it
-
might be to learn certain tasks,
-
just like is the case with
classical conditioning.
-
The final learning
that we'll talk about
-
today is observational learning.
-
This is the ways in
which an organism
-
might learn by observing
-
the experiences of others rather
-
than through his or
her own experience.
-
This could occur
through reading a book,
-
reading a textbook, watching TV,
-
learning vicariously
through others,
-
but it could also
be through learning
-
of your parents'
own experiences,
-
your older siblings experiences.
-
The theories of
observational learning
-
became more important
as the influence
-
of behaviorism explained
through the 1960s and 70s,
-
and important concepts include
-
modeling and vicarious
conditioning.
-
With modeling, we
have this picture
-
which perfectly illustrates it.
-
The dad is shaving,
and of course,
-
a little kid starts
shaving as well.
-
This is when you learn
through imitation.
-
Observing and imitating
the behaviors of others is
-
an important part of the way
that human beings interact.
-
It's the way that we
learn the social norms
-
of our society.
-
It's the way that
we learn what is
-
acceptable and unacceptable,
-
or what are acceptable versus
unacceptable behaviors.
-
Imitation also facilitates
empathy between people.
-
It helps people understand and
-
care about how
other people feel.
-
It's putting yourself
in that person's shoes.
-
Vicarious conditioning occurs on
-
the other hand when
observation of
-
rewarding or punishing
consequences experienced by
-
others serves much
the same function as
-
being rewarded or
punished one's self.
-
For example, when you
were in high school,
-
you saw that people
that were ahead of you,
-
if they got good grades,
-
they got into college,
-
they got scholarships, you might
-
learn through observation
that their behavior,
-
studying a lot and
-
getting these good
grades led to rewards,
-
getting into college and
getting scholarships,
-
that might affect
your own behavior.
-
When it comes to
vicarious conditioning,
-
four steps are required
or four stages are
-
required for you to vicariously
become conditioned.
-
First, we have attention.
-
For observational
learning to occur,
-
one must first direct
one's attention to
-
a model's behavior
and to the rewards
-
or punishments that
follow from the behavior.
-
Let's say that this was
me when I was a sophomore
-
and I saw juniors and seniors.
-
I saw the groups of
kids who studied
-
a lot and got admitted to
-
college and got scholarships
-
versus the kids who
didn't study very much,
-
who often ditched
class and didn't
-
get scholarships are
accepted to college.
-
First, I'm paying attention to
-
those two groups of people.
-
Then you have retention.
-
Because observational
learning involves
-
behaving in the present as
-
someone else has
behaved in the past,
-
one must retain a memory
of the observed behavior.
-
Of course, I'm a
sophomore in high school,
-
so I have to keep
-
my behavior up through
junior and senior years,
-
so I make sure I don't
teach classes too much,
-
make sure that I study
and keep a high GPA so
-
that I can get to college
and get scholarships.
-
It requires some
retention of information.
-
Reproduction, you
must be physically
-
capable of reproducing
the behavior.
-
It must have been
physically capable of
-
attaining a high GPA to
-
get into college and to
get some scholarships.
-
Apart from that, really
important component
-
here is that you have
to have the motivation.
-
You might still be
motivated to perform it.
-
I still have the motivation to
-
perform at very high
levels in academics.
-
That's what led me
-
to get into college
and get scholarships.
-
There are other instances where
-
vicarious conditioning might not
-
occur because you might not be
-
motivated or you might
not be able to reproduce.
-
For example, another example
where I fail to do this is,
-
I always love the
summer Olympics,
-
especially women's gymnastics.
-
First, I paid attention.
-
I looked at the
model's behavior,
-
I watched all over the
summer gymnastics,
-
I learned how to do
cartwheels and all things on
-
my own because I love
gymnastics so much.
-
I retained the information,
-
so I must retain a memory of
the observed behavior I did.
-
That's why I went
and did car wheels
-
and learned all these
things on my own.
-
But then the problem came
down to reproduction.
-
I wasn't physically capable of
-
reproducing all of the
behavior on my own.
-
I required coaching.
-
Maybe it was 10 years too late.
-
Most gymnasts start early on in
-
their lives so that they
retain their flexibility.
-
Because I wasn't physically
-
capable of reproducing
the behavior,
-
it decreased my motivation,
-
so I was no longer
motivated to perform it.
-
Therefore, vicarious
conditioning was not successful.
-
On the next page, I'm
going to show you
-
really it's just such
an adorable picture.
-
This is illustrating
mirror neurons.
-
Mirror neurons describe
a neuron that fires
-
when an animal performs
a particular action,
-
and also when the
animal observes
-
the action performed
by someone else.
-
This person's mirror
neurons are being
-
activated along with this
little monkey's mirror neurons.
-
Mirror neurons allow
the brain to create
-
mental representations
of observed behavior,
-
allowing such behavior
to be learned.
-
This person is holding
a three day old monkey,
-
this baby is only three days old
-
and mimicry or
modeling is really
-
important for creatures to
-
know how to behave
in the wild right.
-
This monkey is observing
-
this human sticking
their tongue out,
-
only three days old,
-
but their mirror neurons
are already being
-
activated only at
three days old,
-
this little monkey learns
how to stick his tongue,
-
his or her tongue out too.
-
This is super adorable.
-
I'll post a video that
shows you more about
-
the different
behaviors that they
-
were able to elicit from
this three day old monkey.
-
The final learning, well,
-
I guess this follows,
-
modeling and
observational learning.
-
Observational learning
has often been
-
used as a core argument
-
for the relationship between
media violence and behavior.
-
Strong opinions exist
regarding the question of
-
effects of violent media,
violent video games,
-
violent TV shows,
violent websites,
-
et cetera, on aggression.
-
But the question hasn't really
-
been settled to
everyone's satisfaction.
-
There are some studies
that show that there's
-
a link between media violence,
-
and aggressive thoughts,
emotions, and behavior.
-
But this association isn't
always straightforward.
-
There is a difference
between possible effects of
-
violent media on aggression
and the effects of violence,
-
but there are all confounds
that exist within that.
-
Are people that already have
-
violent tendencies more likely
to watch violent media?
-
Probably, even with the
absence of violent media,
-
would these people engage
in violent behavior?
-
Maybe. It's not only
-
the fact that violent media
leads to aggressive behavior.
-
There are other factors like
-
personality factors,
the context,
-
the social environment in
which these people grow up
-
in that leads people to
engage in violent behavior.
-
It's not only
observational learning,
-
but other factors contribute
to violent behavior.
-
I will post this
video up and then
-
I will share the
other keep video
-
with mirror neurons
and the baby monkeys.
-
Enjoy the rest of your day
and I'll see you soon.