-
>> Good day and welcome IB psychologists to
another video tutorial with me, Travis Dixon.
-
Today, we're going to look
at social cognitive theory.
-
We're going to do a full summary.
-
It's probably going to take
about 27 minutes I think
-
because I made this video
yesterday, but my mic was turned off.
-
So, anyway, it'll take about 30 minutes.
-
We're going do a full summary here
so you've got everything you need
-
for your IB psychology exams.
-
By the end of this video, you're
going to be able to do a full summary
-
of Bandura's social cognitive theory.
-
This is a core part of the
IB psychology curriculum,
-
so you must know about social cognitive theory.
-
And you'll be able to evaluate
SCT using the D.E.A.L. structure.
-
I've used this in my other videos about
schema theory and social identity theory,
-
and it's a pretty good framework to
remember how to evaluate theories.
-
First of all, we're going to describe it, what
is the theory, how does it explain behavior.
-
Then, we'll look at some studies that support
it, applications, followed by limitations.
-
Your studies and your applications
are really the strengths of the theory
-
and your limitations there, so
you've got the full evaluation.
-
Just a note, this is also
covered in my revision guide.
-
Where is my copy?
-
It's buried down here somewhere.
-
So, if you've got the revision guide, then
this is all in there, but if you don't have it
-
and maybe it's too close to your exams to
get it, everything's in this video for you.
-
So, first of all, the description
of social cognitive theory.
-
So, this is Albert Bandura.
-
This was his theory.
-
We're going to look at it
in four major parts really.
-
First, we'll have a little bit of background.
-
And then, there's three major
concepts that you should know about.
-
At least the first two are really
key, observational learning
-
and triadic reciprocal determinism.
-
These are the two most important factors I
think of SCT, and there's also the concept
-
of self-efficacy, which we'll look at as well.
-
So, a little bit of background on the theory.
-
So, this is as I said proposed by Bandura.
-
The original theory was called
social learning theory,
-
and this was first proposed in the 1960s.
-
The core claim of social learning
theory was that we can learn behavior,
-
behavior can be explained by looking at how
we learn from others, we learn by observation.
-
And it's important I think whenever we
look at a theory to look at the context.
-
So, what was that theory in relation to?
-
The theory of evolution was in
contrast to the theory of creation.
-
Social learning theory was in contrast
to behaviorism saying that we learn
-
by either being rewarded or
punished for our behavior.
-
So, it's our direct actions, which we then
get rewarded for, we'll keep doing it,
-
if we get punished, we'll stop doing it.
-
And Bandura said that's not
actually the only way we can learn.
-
And also, this is Watson here for behaviorism.
-
And also, Freud's idea was that our behavior
comes from these deep internal motivations,
-
maybe childhood experiences that would repressed
and they're coming out in our behavior.
-
And again, Bandura was saying maybe that's
not the only explanation for behavior.
-
That might not always be the case.
-
Anyway, we have Freud as well who we don't
actually study much in IB psychology.
-
So, let's break down observational learning.
-
This is the core part of social learning theory.
-
Observational learning is one of the
key concepts of social cognitive theory.
-
He renamed in social cognitive theory.
-
He renamed it to social cognitive
theory in about the 1980s.
-
So, in the original SLT, Bandura said we
can learn from two ways, direct experience.
-
I put my hand on a fire, that's
hot, I learn not to do that.
-
But I can also learn by observing others.
-
And Bandura said we don't actually always have
to have direct experience to learn something.
-
This going to be dangerous.
-
I don't always have to be putting my hand
on hot fires to learn that they're hot.
-
I can see someone else put their hand on a hot
fire and go, "Wow, they got burned from that.
-
Now I know not to do it."
-
It's the old monkey see, monkey do.
-
And so, observational learning
we can break down.
-
There are four process is involved.
-
And this is the key part of this concept in
social cognitive theory, that there's attention.
-
We're paying attention to the model.
-
So, there's the observer and there's the model.
-
The model is the person performing the action.
-
So, the learner has to pay attention.
-
If they don't, they're not going to remember it.
-
And they have to remember it.
-
They have to retain it.
-
This is what retention refers to.
-
Mode of reproduction, we have to
physically be able to reproduce the skill.
-
If we can't physically do it, then the
chances of us copying it and imitating it
-
and later learning it aren't
going to be very high.
-
For example, this happens a lot in sports.
-
Many years ago as a basketball
player, I learned how to dribble
-
and put the ball behind my back
by watching someone else do it.
-
And I was like, "Man, that's cool."
-
And so, then I went off and tried and
tried and tried until I could do it.
-
Now, I also watched someone put the ball
between their legs and slam dunk it.
-
I couldn't do that.
-
I never learned how to do that.
-
I don't have the motor skills.
-
Reinforcement and motivation, the final
process involved here, and this can be broken
-
down a little bit further as well.
-
But reinforcement, if we are watching someone
and they are getting rewarded or punished
-
for their behavior, we're
more likely to copy it.
-
And if we identify with their model,
if we find some sort of similarity,
-
we're probably more likely to be
motivated to copy their learning.
-
And this becomes really important
when we look at --
-
it's not just in-person modeling
that we're looking at.
-
We're also looking at media and the
effects of how we can see models
-
and imitate behavior we see
on TV and other places.
-
So, that's observational learning.
-
So, a good summary of social
cognitive theory would include saying
-
that behavior can be explained by
how we learn it by observing others
-
and include these four processes
in that summary.
-
Now, the key concept is triadic
reciprocal determinism.
-
This is also called triadic
reciprocal causation.
-
And what this simply means is -- this
is a pretty fancy and complicated term
-
for I think a pretty straightforward
concept that our environment,
-
our physical external environment influences our
individual internal factors like our cognition
-
and our biology and that
can affect our behavior.
-
And actually, all these three
things can influence one another.
-
It's triadic.
-
There's three.
-
It's reciprocal meaning it goes both ways.
-
And they can determine one another.
-
They can cause one another.
-
So, that's essentially it.
-
So, if we think of one example of
neuroplasticity, we know if we grow
-
up in a low socioeconomic environment,
-
we're going to have possibly
smaller brain development.
-
There's correlational studies that showed this.
-
So, possibly growing up in poverty we're
going to have a smaller hippocampus.
-
That's our internal factor.
-
That might affect another
internal factor, our memory,
-
because hippocampus is part of our memory.
-
Now, that could affect our behavior.
-
If you don't have very good memory, it means
you're going to be maybe not good at learning.
-
You might not do very well at school.
-
So, then, you'll start having behavior
problems and behavior issues, acting out.
-
And this is what happens a lot.
-
But we can actually also reverse this trend.
-
You can do lots of things.
-
For example, playing some
kinds of video games we've seen
-
in studies can help boost the
development in your hippocampus.
-
And maybe that might make
your classroom a better place.
-
I mean, going from the individual
to the environment,
-
this link here is a little bit harder to make.
-
And going this direction and
this direction is a bit tricky.
-
But anyway, you can see they can
possibly influence one another.
-
Another example, we look at the
warrior gene and aggression.
-
So, we know that adverse childhood experiences
-- so, growing up if you're abused --
-
is one example that can influence
the expression of your genes,
-
your biological development,
and your brain development.
-
It might also influence the warrior
gene, and that can affect our behavior.
-
It might make you more likely to be antisocial.
-
And this can be seen in Caspi, et al.'s
study, which we'll look at a little bit later.
-
So, triadic reciprocal determinism
I think is a key --
-
most students I think will write when
they talk about social cognitive theory,
-
they'll focus on observational learning
and they'll forget this key detail.
-
But if you add this core concept to
a summary of social cognitive theory,
-
I really think it's going to
separate your answer from the rest
-
and including this diagram here
would be a very good idea as well.
-
Finally, we get to self-efficacy.
-
Self-efficacy means your belief
in your ability to do something.
-
And so, it's kind of like self-confidence
and how well you think you can do something.
-
And Bandura said that this is an
important internal factor over here.
-
They can moderate this relationship.
-
Take one example, for example,
your academic self-efficacy.
-
How well do you think that
you can do well in school?
-
And this is often correlated with achievement.
-
Those students who have a high self-efficacy
in terms of academics and think they are good
-
at learning and good at studying,
they tend to do better at school.
-
And that makes sense.
-
You think about it with your IB psych exam.
-
How confident are you in your ability
to learn social cognitive theory?
-
How well do you think you're going to be able
to learn and remember this lecture and then
-
that my influence how well you can summarize it.
-
So, this is self-efficacy.
-
There's not a lot to say.
-
I didn't include it in my original book just
because I think with the first two concepts,
-
triadic reciprocal determinism and observational
learning, you've got more than enough
-
for social cognitive theory, but
I thought I would edit it here
-
and I did also put it in
the revision book as well.
-
It's pretty straightforward.
-
So, that's the description,
those three concepts.
-
Now, the evidence, the supporting studies.
-
First of all, we can't go past the
Bobo doll studies I don't think
-
because these are very famous
and we'd be remiss if we went
-
through an introductory IB psych course
and we didn't cover the Bobo doll studies.
-
And for observational learning,
I think they work really well.
-
So, I'm not going to go into heaps of detail
about the methodology because I'll put a link
-
in the description where you can find a
summary on my blog of one of these studies.
-
And also, there's just so much out there and
you've probably already seen the videos anyway.
-
But anyway, in the first study in 1961,
they had two independent variables.
-
One was the gender of the model and the other
was what type of observation the kids did.
-
So, I'm sure you know about it.
-
The kids came in.
-
They either watched an adult playing
aggressively with the Bobo doll
-
or they watched them playing passively.
-
And the model was either the
same gender or different gender.
-
And then, they were put in a room and
there was a couple of researchers.
-
They stood behind the two-way mirror
-
and they counted every 5 seconds was the
kid making an aggressive action or not,
-
and they recorded that, and
then they compared their notes.
-
Just a couple of details
here taken from the original.
-
If you can put some specific
details in your exam answers,
-
it will show that you really know your stuff
and it will separate your answers from the rest.
-
So, 36 boys, 36 girls.
-
So, what's that?
-
About 3 to 5 years old from Stanford University.
-
And two adults.
-
So, male, one female were in the male position.
-
I've never seen videos of the male.
-
I've only ever seen videos of the female.
-
But anyway, now, we have the original
results here taken from the original study.
-
And we're just looking over here.
-
This is one of the important results.
-
So, this is physical aggression.
-
So, if we just compare this, the aggressive
conditions over here we see quite high compared
-
to the non-aggressive and the control groups.
-
It's much lower over here.
-
And you might want to get a pen and paper.
-
Excuse me.
-
I'm losing my voice.
-
You might want to get a pen and paper
and just jot down a couple of these notes
-
so you've got some specifics to refer to.
-
But if we just look here, let's just
look at the males to begin with.
-
Well, one interesting trend is generally
speaking the boys were more aggressive
-
than the females.
-
And also, if we look here, this first
result shows observational learning,
-
that if we view aggressive behavior,
we're more likely to copy it.
-
Straightforward.
-
This is evidence to support
observational learning.
-
There's also some evidence here to support
the fact that we might be more motivated
-
to copy the learning if we
identify with the model.
-
We see here especially with verbal
aggression the females were far more likely
-
to copy the verbal aggression
than if they watched a male,
-
when they watched a female,
and opposite for the males.
-
If they watched a male, far more likely
to copy it than if they watched a female.
-
So, there's some evidence.
-
This here is observational learning in general.
-
And then, this here we can see the identifying
with the model we might be more motivated.
-
Now, second study in 1963.
-
This is one I have the full
summary for on the blog,
-
and I'll work at getting the
1961 version up there as well.
-
They wanted to see does it have to be
real life observation of aggression
-
or can it be by seeing a televised version?
-
And so, they used the same procedures as the
first study, but it was a little bit different
-
in how the kids actually viewed the model.
-
So, they had one in-person live model, so the
kids actually are in the same room watching.
-
They had one where they video recorded
the model beating up the Bobo doll
-
and then they put that on the TV.
-
Or they had the televised fantasy model.
-
This was a really interesting one where they
wanted to make it appear like a cartoon,
-
but I guess they probably couldn't afford
to create a cartoon just for these purposes.
-
This is 50 or 60 years ago, so it would
have been a bit expensive back then.
-
So, they had the researcher dress as a cat.
-
And then, they tried to make the scenery like
with artificial grass and make it kind of appear
-
like a fantasy land and they had a
control group that didn't watch anything.
-
Again, this is summarized on my blog there.
-
You can check it out and I'll
put the link in the description.
-
So, what do we see?
-
Again, here are the original
results taken from the first study.
-
The real life aggressive condition, so
when they actually saw the real person,
-
this is the filmed, this is the cartoon,
-
although it wasn't really
cartoon, and the control group.
-
What we can see again, the control
group much lower than the other three.
-
More evidence to suggest and to support
the idea of observational learning,
-
that we can learn aggressive
behavior through observation.
-
And I just want to point out here that
social cognitive theory can be used
-
to explain many behaviors.
-
I think using it to explain aggression can
really help you write a good focused explanation
-
of this theory.
-
And that was one of its first applications.
-
So, when you're thinking about how
to explain a theory, think about,
-
"What is this theory explaining?"
-
Aggression.
-
So, back to it.
-
So, again, observational learning here as well.
-
And what's interesting is generally
speaking again, the boys more aggressive
-
than the females, which is an
interesting trend and something we'll talk
-
about later when we get to the evaluation.
-
And so, they didn't actually notice that
this was that much higher than, for example,
-
watching it in real life, but it does
actually mean that it's comparable
-
and it's much higher watching the cartoon
and the film version than the control group.
-
So, this is evidence to suggest that it's
not just watching someone in real life,
-
but we can also be influenced and we can learn
-
by watching filmed televised
models of aggression.
-
Now, this might seem just everyday
knowledge to us, but this is 1963.
-
This is when TV was first becoming
introduced to many households in America.
-
And so, that becomes a very important
finding when we put it in that context,
-
that we don't actually have to go visit
and see a boxing match, for example,
-
to copy that aggressive behavior, but we can be
seeing it every day in our living rooms on TV.
-
And so, in terms of developmental psychology
with kids and how much TV we watch,
-
this becomes a very important
finding especially at this time.
-
So, those studies I think are really good to
show the first concept, observational learning.
-
Now, we need some examples to show
triadic reciprocal determinism.
-
Now, you're not going to find I don't think
many studies that show all three interactions
-
and I think you can actually do a good job of
just explaining it and giving some examples
-
like maybe with a couple of studies
like I did with neuroplasticity
-
and showing the environment affecting the brain.
-
Like [inaudible] is a good one to show that
and then links between the brain and behavior,
-
any study that shows links between
hippocampus and memory like maybe HM.
-
But sometimes, you get a study
like Caspi's study that we can use
-
to show those three interacting elements.
-
So, this was also called the Dunedin study.
-
It followed 1,000 people for 25 years,
and they were looking at links between --
-
in this particular report, they looked at links
between the type of warrior gene or the type
-
of MAOA gene that people had
with antisocial behavior.
-
And they compared two groups of people.
-
One group had the low-expression
variant, which means the gene,
-
this variant is not sending
many signals from the cell.
-
This is what gene expression is.
-
And the other was the high-expression variant.
-
Now, what they found was that the having the
MAOA-L variant or one of those variants --
-
and this is the type of the
warrior gene that is most associated
-
with antisocial and aggressive behavior.
-
They found that that alone doesn't necessarily
have an effect on antisocial behavior.
-
But if you combine that with childhood
abuse, we can see the result here.
-
So, this is the low activity in
black and high activity of the gene.
-
Now, just as biological factor by
itself, not much of a difference
-
if there's no childhood maltreatment.
-
But if we get severe childhood maltreatment,
we'll see this low-expression variant,
-
the levels of antisocial behavior,
which is over here, skyrockets.
-
And this is showing that that effect of the
environment might be influencing the expression
-
of our genetics or some internal factors and
then that's having a result in our behavior.
-
And we can talk about one area of uncertainty
here is what is it actually doing to our brain
-
or our genetics or why is this difference
occurring, but we can see here the effect
-
of environment is influencing internal
factors in some way, which is resulting
-
in this massive increase in behavior.
-
So, finally, self-efficacy and GPA.
-
So, one basic study to look at
the third concept, self-efficacy,
-
and this is often studied in correlational
studies where you take two factors
-
like academics and you measure
people's self-efficacy on their beliefs
-
and their abilities to do or to be good
at academics and their achievement.
-
And then, you see how strongly
they're correlated.
-
And this is what they did.
-
In this study, 60 students studying sports
science in Malaysia and they took their GPAs
-
for the university scores and then they
also measured their academic self-efficacy.
-
They found very high correlations here.
-
Males 0.67.
-
Females 0.85.
-
We think a plus one is a perfect correlation.
-
Zero is no correlation.
-
So, anything with a 0.6 or 0.7 is pretty strong.
-
So, 0.8, that's a really strong
correlation seen here in females.
-
So, evidence to suggest that higher
self-efficacy is going to influence behavior.
-
Again, this could be further evidence maybe
for at least two of those interactions
-
in triadic reciprocal determinism.
-
So, there are studies.
-
Now, we get to the A and D.E.A.L. applications.
-
This can explain violence in the media.
-
There's been lots of studies that have shown
the more aggressive violent media you watch
-
as a kid the more likely you
are to act out and be aggressive
-
and end up in court yourself later in life.
-
And so, social cognitive theory
can be used to explain that,
-
that the idea of observational
learning we can learn through the media.
-
But with that, we can also apply
this to learning in a couple of ways.
-
One is use of modeling as
an instructional strategy.
-
You want to teach someone how to do
something, you show them how to do it first.
-
I've got a son who's three years old.
-
We do this all the time.
-
I'm trying to teach him how to do
something, tie his shoelaces, I'll do it,
-
he sees it, and then he tries to copy it.
-
He's not learning that yet, but I couldn't
think of a better example off top of my head.
-
I'll think of one in a couple of minutes.
-
The other way, if we can use the
idea of self-efficacy and if we know
-
that that might be linked to
improved performance and achievement,
-
then maybe we can develop strategies
and there have been strategies developed
-
to increase self-efficacy
especially in the likes of academics.
-
Also, if we can use social cognitive theory
and the idea of observational learning
-
to explain how we can learn negative behaviors
through the media, we could also use that
-
and it has been used to help develop positive
behaviors, and this is called the Sabido method
-
where there's been scripts and stories that are
designed to teach some kind of life lessons.
-
This was done a lot in South and Central
America with tackling social issues
-
by having it play out with the characters.
-
But also if you're familiar
with, for example, Sesame Street,
-
nearly every episode they are
teaching kids about sharing
-
or learning and using puppets to do that.
-
And so, we can actually apply these
concepts of observation learning
-
to promote positive behavior
as well in television shows.
-
So, there are some applications
of social cognitive theory.
-
Remember, you wouldn't put this in a
short answer response in your exams,
-
but you would put this in the essay.
-
Finally, we get to some limitations.
-
Evaluating theories is really, really
tricky, so let's do our best here.
-
First of all, are there alternative
explanations?
-
So, if we're applying social
cognitive theory to explain aggression,
-
are there alternative explanations
for aggression
-
or are there other factors
that might be involved here?
-
Can we say that aggressive behavior is 100%
learned or like I said are there other factors
-
like our hormones, our genetics,
our culture, things like this?
-
Are there other factors that might influence --
-
actually, I'm just thinking of
this off the top of my head now.
-
Observational learning, what
other factors might influence?
-
Are some people more likely to copy than others?
-
Are there some internal factors that
might make some people more likely
-
to replicate and copy other people?
-
But I guess he also covered that in his
motivation like identifying with the model.
-
Anyway. Ecological validity
of the Bobo doll studies.
-
Now, ideally, you want to evaluate the
theory before you evaluate the studies,
-
but I think here because the Bobo studies are so
prominent, I think it's okay to evaluate these.
-
And if we think about the Bobo doll
studies, is it really the case in real life
-
where a kid's watching television and they're
watching an adult play with or perform some sort
-
of aggressive violent act and then the kid
turns off the TV and turns around and voila,
-
everything that they just saw on the
television is there in front of them to copy?
-
That's what happened in the Bobo doll study.
-
There's a big inflatable doll
they're watching with a hammer.
-
And then, they turn around
and that same doll is there.
-
This is not what happens in real life.
-
We watch cartoons and there's big anvils,
metal things drop on people's heads
-
or there's guns or there's cowboys and Indians.
-
But then, we turn around and they're not there.
-
So, that transfer, could this really
explain that learning to be aggressive
-
and then transferring it in a different context?
-
Questionable.
-
We saw throughout that there was a higher
increase from the boys in aggression
-
and can social cognitive theory explain this?
-
Is this just because maybe boys
are taught more to be aggressive
-
or is there some biological factors?
-
It's the old nature versus nurture here.
-
Why are men generally more aggressive?
-
And then, we come back to, for
example, maybe testosterone.
-
Is this a factor or is this just because
boys are raised to be more aggressive?
-
Evaluating the concept of TRD,
isn't this just common knowledge?
-
We now just know in psychology after years --
-
I mean, in the 1980s when this idea was
first added into social cognitive theory,
-
it might have been groundbreaking, like the
idea that our environment can affect our genes.
-
But now, we just know that.
-
We know from epigenetic studies and we
know that environment affects our brain.
-
It seems like a redundant point in this day
and age I think at least because it's just
-
so well known that the nature or
nurture debate is largely dead
-
and we know that it's nature or nurture.
-
Sometimes, it's nature via
nurture and vice versa.
-
So, I think that's one limitation of that idea.
-
Self-efficacy.
-
Now, maybe I'm showing my ignorance on this,
-
but I'm wondering how we could really
genuinely test this experimentally.
-
I think mostly it's going
to be correlational studies
-
because I mean how can we
ethically manipulate people's levels
-
of self-efficacy unless we
did it in their self-efficacy
-
in a field that doesn't really matter?
-
But then, why would we care about that?
-
So, for example, do you want to take one
group and reduce yourself in academics
-
and see if it has an effect or maybe
we could take one group and improve it
-
and boost your self-efficacy and
see if that has an effect as well,
-
but there's maybe an ethical
-- possibly it could be done.
-
Anyway. For self-efficacy, you could
I think evaluate the studies that show
-
that this is correlational and it might
be tricky to study experimentally.
-
But if you know of any experiments
on self-efficacy,
-
true experiments where you manipulate levels
of self-efficacy, post it in the comments.
-
I'd love to learn about them.
-
That's it.
-
Full on. I reckon I've probably
gone over 30 minutes.
-
I don't have a timer.
-
So, hopefully, you can describe
social cognitive theory,
-
you can provide the studies
as evidence to support it.
-
Remember, a short answer response you would
just summarize the theory and have one study.
-
If you are going into an essay, you'd
want to have two or three studies.
-
And hopefully, you can explain at
least one or two of those applications.
-
I think it's a pretty easy one to explain,
-
especially things like Sesame Street
observation learning on television.
-
And then, at least a couple
of limitations as well.
-
So, a couple of exam tips before we leave.
-
Short answer questions, describe
social cognitive theory
-
as a possible question you might get,
and it's one that's been asked before.
-
In order to do this and answer this properly,
explain at least two of those core claims,
-
maybe all three, but you want to
give as much detail as possible.
-
A common mistake students make is they'll have
one sentence or two sentences about the theory.
-
Any question about a theory whether it's a model
of memory or if it's social identity theory,
-
schema theory, a really, really common mistake
that students make is they'll spend one
-
or two sentences mentioning the theory and
then the rest of the answer is about the study.
-
You'll never get full marks doing that.
-
You have to have a full description,
a full summary of the theory.
-
Make it nice and concise.
-
It should be about 150 words of a
summary using all the right terminology
-
and then get into the study.
-
Use one study in depth.
-
And most importantly, show how that
study supports an aspect of the theory.
-
So, if you're using the Bobo doll study,
you'll take a couple of specific results
-
and explain how that demonstrates
observational learning.
-
If you're talking about Caspi's study,
you'll look at those specific results
-
and how that supports the idea of
triadic reciprocal determinism.
-
So, it's important that you link the specific
result to one of the claims of the theory,
-
and that's another common thing
that's missing in student answers.
-
If you're writing an essay,
discuss equals evaluate.
-
I haven't yet been convinced
that this means otherwise.
-
So, if you're asked to discuss social
cognitive theory, it means to evaluate.
-
Follow the D.E.A.L. structure.
-
Describe it.
-
Give the studies, the applications,
the limitations,
-
and the evaluation is the same thing.
-
Evaluate social cognitive theory,
discuss social cognitive theory,
-
exact same answers would
both get excellent marks.
-
Now, again, just a reminder, try to have
limitations of the theory to begin with,
-
at least one or two, and then if you've
got more time evaluate the studies.
-
But be careful with how you
evaluate the studies.
-
Methodology of the studies, yes.
-
But ethics, like if you're going to
evaluate social cognitive theory and you talk
-
about ethical limitations of the Bobo doll
studies with teaching kids to be aggressive,
-
it's a pretty loose point to make.
-
So, you really want to be
evaluating the theory first
-
and then maybe the validity of the studies.
-
Ethicality is not so relevant.
-
So, hopefully, you can use social cognitive
theory to explain behavior like aggression,
-
you can use at least one
key study that supports it,
-
and you can explain strengths and limitations.
-
I really hope that was helpful.
-
Leave comments if it was or if it
wasn't if you got ways I can improve.
-
Subscribe to our blog.
-
Heaps of stuff over there, and
I keep trying to update it.
-
I've just finished uploading
all the exam banks that I have,
-
so you can see some possible
exam questions there.
-
I'm new to Twitter, but I'm trying
to get out one exam tip every day.
-
So, the link to that is in
the description as well.
-
That might help.
-
And we've got Facebook groups
and everything else.
-
Everything is in the description
that you need including our books.
-
So, good luck.
-
If you've made it this far, well done.
-
And if you're a student and you made it this
far, I can tell that you've very motivated
-
to do well in your exams, so
best of luck and let me know
-
if there's anything I can do to help you out.
-
Cheers.