< Return to Video

The "End of History" Illusion - Bence Nanay

  • 0:08 - 0:11
    When trains began to shuttle people
    across the coutryside,
  • 0:11 - 0:14
    many insisted
    they would never replace horses.
  • 0:14 - 0:19
    Less than a century later, people
    repeated that same prediction about cars,
  • 0:19 - 0:20
    telephones,
  • 0:20 - 0:21
    radio,
  • 0:21 - 0:22
    television,
  • 0:22 - 0:23
    and computers.
  • 0:23 - 0:25
    Each had their own host of detractors.
  • 0:25 - 0:29
    Even some experts
    insisted they wouldn’t catch on.
  • 0:29 - 0:33
    Of course, we can’t predict exactly
    what the future will look like
  • 0:33 - 0:35
    or what new inventions will populate it.
  • 0:35 - 0:37
    But time and time again,
  • 0:37 - 0:40
    we’ve also failed to predict
    that the technologies of the present
  • 0:40 - 0:42
    will change the future.
  • 0:42 - 0:47
    And recent research has revealed
    a similar pattern in our individual lives:
  • 0:47 - 0:51
    we’re unable to predict change
    in ourselves.
  • 0:51 - 0:56
    Three psychologists documented
    our inability to predict personal change
  • 0:56 - 1:00
    in a 2013 paper
    called, “The End of History Illusion.”
  • 1:00 - 1:04
    Named after political scientist
    Francis Fukuyama’s prediction
  • 1:04 - 1:07
    that liberal democracy
    was the final form of government,
  • 1:07 - 1:10
    or as he called it, “the end of history,”
  • 1:10 - 1:14
    their work highlights the way
    we see ourselves as finished products
  • 1:14 - 1:16
    at any given moment.
  • 1:16 - 1:22
    The researchers recruited over
    7,000 participants ages 18 to 68.
  • 1:22 - 1:26
    They asked half of these participants to
    report their current personality traits,
  • 1:26 - 1:27
    values,
  • 1:27 - 1:29
    and preferences,
  • 1:29 - 1:33
    along with what each of those metrics
    had been ten years before.
  • 1:33 - 1:37
    The other half described those features
    in their present selves,
  • 1:37 - 1:41
    and predicted what
    they would be ten years in the future.
  • 1:41 - 1:42
    Based on these answers,
  • 1:42 - 1:45
    the researchers then calculated
    the degree of change
  • 1:45 - 1:49
    each participant reported or predicted.
  • 1:49 - 1:51
    For every age group in the sample,
  • 1:51 - 1:54
    they compared the predicted changes
    to the reported changes.
  • 1:54 - 1:59
    So they compared the degree to which
    18-year-olds thought they would change
  • 1:59 - 2:03
    to the degree to which 28-year-olds
    reported they had changed.
  • 2:03 - 2:05
    Overwhelmingly, at all ages,
  • 2:05 - 2:09
    people’s future estimates of change
    came up short
  • 2:09 - 2:13
    compared to the changes
    their older counterparts recalled.
  • 2:13 - 2:17
    20-year-olds expected
    to still like the same foods at 30,
  • 2:17 - 2:20
    but 30-year-olds no longer
    had the same tastes.
  • 2:20 - 2:25
    30-year-olds predicted they’d still
    have the same best friend at 40,
  • 2:25 - 2:28
    but 40-year-olds
    had lost touch with theirs.
  • 2:28 - 2:32
    And 40-year-olds predicted
    they’d maintain the same core values
  • 2:32 - 2:35
    that 50-year-olds had reconsidered.
  • 2:35 - 2:39
    While older people changed less
    than younger people on the whole,
  • 2:39 - 2:44
    they underestimated
    their capacity for change just as much.
  • 2:44 - 2:48
    Wherever we are in life,
    the end of history illusion persists:
  • 2:48 - 2:53
    we tend to think that the bulk
    of our personal change is behind us.
  • 2:53 - 2:55
    One consequence of this thinking
  • 2:55 - 2:58
    is that we’re inclined to overinvest
    in future choices
  • 2:58 - 3:01
    based on present preferences.
  • 3:01 - 3:04
    On average, people are willing
    to pay about 60% more
  • 3:04 - 3:08
    to see their current favorite musician
    ten years in the future
  • 3:08 - 3:12
    than they’d currently pay to see their
    favorite musician from ten years ago.
  • 3:12 - 3:16
    While the stakes involved
    in concert-going are low,
  • 3:16 - 3:18
    we’re susceptible
    to similar miscalculations
  • 3:18 - 3:20
    in more serious commitments,
  • 3:20 - 3:21
    like homes,
  • 3:21 - 3:22
    partners,
  • 3:22 - 3:24
    and jobs.
  • 3:24 - 3:26
    At the same time,
    there’s no real way to predict
  • 3:26 - 3:29
    what our preferences
    will be in the future.
  • 3:29 - 3:31
    Without the end of history Illusion,
  • 3:31 - 3:34
    it would be difficult
    to make any long-term plans.
  • 3:34 - 3:38
    So the end of history illusion
    applies to our individual lives,
  • 3:38 - 3:40
    but what about the wider world?
  • 3:40 - 3:45
    Could we be assuming that how things
    are now is how they will continue to be?
  • 3:45 - 3:48
    If so, fortunately,
    there are countless records
  • 3:48 - 3:52
    to remind us that the world does change,
    sometimes for the better.
  • 3:52 - 3:55
    Our own historical moment
    isn’t the end of history,
  • 3:55 - 3:59
    and that can be just as much a source
    of comfort as a cause for concern.
Title:
The "End of History" Illusion - Bence Nanay
Speaker:
Bence Nanay
Description:

Time and time again, we’ve failed to predict that the technologies of the present will change the future. Recently, a similar pattern was discovered in our individual lives: we’re unable to predict change in ourselves. But is there anything wrong with assuming that who we are now is who we will continue to be? Bence Nanay details the consequences of seeing ourselves as finished products.

Lesson by Bence Nanay, directed by Stretch Films Inc.

more » « less
Video Language:
English
Team:
closed TED
Project:
TED-Ed
Duration:
04:15

English subtitles

Revisions Compare revisions