< Return to Video

Re-evaluating The Marshmallow Test

  • 0:00 - 0:05
    [school bell rings]
  • 0:05 - 0:09
    -If I eat 100 marshmallows,
    I would be filled up!
  • 0:09 - 0:11
    (Richard Aslin)
    Everybody knows that young children,
  • 0:11 - 0:14
    particularly toddlers--
    two to four-year-olds--
  • 0:14 - 0:17
    are subject to impulsivity,
  • 0:17 - 0:20
    and it has been thought that
    that is a characteristic
  • 0:20 - 0:22
    that children are born with.
  • 0:22 - 0:25
    -Do you know what?
    It is snack time now.
  • 0:25 - 0:28
    -And so, what we wanted to know
    is whether or not
  • 0:28 - 0:31
    some of these differences
    between children
  • 0:31 - 0:35
    can be influenced by their own
    rational thought processes.
  • 0:35 - 0:38
    -We wanted to manipulate
    children's beliefs
  • 0:38 - 0:41
    about how reliable the environment
    that they were in was.
  • 0:41 - 0:43
    We assign kids to one of two conditions:
  • 0:43 - 0:46
    either the reliable condition
    or the unreliable condition.
  • 0:46 - 0:48
    -So, for the art supplies
    that you get to use,
  • 0:48 - 0:49
    you actually have a choice.
  • 0:49 - 0:52
    You can either use these crayons
    right now,
  • 0:52 - 0:55
    or if you can wait for me to go get some
    from the other room,
  • 0:55 - 0:58
    you can use our big set of art supplies
    instead.
  • 0:58 - 0:59
    (Celeste Kidd)
    The kids in the reliable condition,
  • 0:59 - 1:01
    when the team member came back
    into the room,
  • 1:01 - 1:04
    had the better thing,
    and for the kids
  • 1:04 - 1:06
    in the unreliable condition,
    the experimenter apologized,
  • 1:06 - 1:09
    and said she made a mistake,
    we didn't have that available,
  • 1:09 - 1:10
    and then helped them use
    the first option.
  • 1:10 - 1:14
    -I'm so sorry, but I actually don't have
    that big set of art supplies
  • 1:14 - 1:16
    I told you about.
    Sorry about that.
  • 1:16 - 1:19
    But you can still use these ones
    to make your project.
  • 1:19 - 1:22
    (Aslin)
    A classic example of a task
  • 1:22 - 1:24
    in which children show impulsivity
  • 1:24 - 1:26
    is what's called
    the "marshmallow task".
  • 1:26 - 1:30
    -For your snack, you have a choice.
    Look what I've got.
  • 1:30 - 1:32
    -[gasp] A marshmallow!
  • 1:32 - 1:34
    -Yeah! So, wait, just a second;
    let me explain.
  • 1:34 - 1:37
    So, you can either eat
    this one marshmallow right now,
  • 1:37 - 1:41
    or if you can wait for me to go get it
    from the other room,
  • 1:41 - 1:43
    you can have two marshmallows instead.
  • 1:43 - 1:46
    -I want two marshmallows!
  • 1:46 - 1:48
    (Aslin)
    And what we found,
  • 1:48 - 1:50
    which was an incredibly large effect,
  • 1:50 - 1:53
    the children who were in
    the unreliable group
  • 1:53 - 1:56
    were more likely to fairly quickly
  • 1:56 - 1:58
    pick up the marshmallow
    and eat it.
  • 1:58 - 2:01
    So, on average,
    they waited about three minutes.
  • 2:01 - 2:04
    -And did you know,
    I did not eat this marshmallow yet?
  • 2:04 - 2:05
    (Aslin) The children
    who were in the reliable group
  • 2:05 - 2:09
    waited four times longer,
    so they waited about 12 minutes,
  • 2:09 - 2:11
    which is an incredibly long time
  • 2:11 - 2:15
    for young children to wait
    before they get a reward.
  • 2:15 - 2:17
    (Kidd) The difference
    is maybe due to differing expectations
  • 2:17 - 2:19
    about what's likely to happen
    in the world.
  • 2:19 - 2:20
    That's what this experiment
    was designed to address.
  • 2:20 - 2:22
    -For any three year old,
    self-control is not necessarily
  • 2:22 - 2:24
    at the top of their skill base.
    [laughs]
  • 2:24 - 2:26
    But, in general,
    when she sets her mind
  • 2:26 - 2:28
    that she's going to do something,
    she's going to do it.
  • 2:28 - 2:30
    (Kidd) In the marshmallow task,
    what you want to do
  • 2:30 - 2:34
    is you want to get
    the most amount of marshmallow possible,
  • 2:34 - 2:36
    but there may be other considerations.
  • 2:36 - 2:39
    Given that I have this one marshmallow
    now that's guaranteed,
  • 2:39 - 2:40
    what are the chances that if I wait,
  • 2:40 - 2:42
    there's going to be a second marshmallow
    later?
  • 2:42 - 2:45
    -If it was a teacher he had
    all the time, that if--
  • 2:45 - 2:48
    they would have that trust bond
    a little bit longer,
  • 2:48 - 2:49
    then he'd probably wait longer.
  • 2:49 - 2:51
    (Aslin)
    One of the lines of work
  • 2:51 - 2:55
    that evolved as a result
    of the marshmallow task
  • 2:55 - 3:00
    was to look at the outcomes
  • 3:00 - 3:03
    of children's behavior
    later in life,
  • 3:03 - 3:05
    but something that's been missing
    from the equation
  • 3:05 - 3:09
    is this rational process
    by which children
  • 3:09 - 3:12
    are accessing information
    in their environment
  • 3:12 - 3:14
    and making decisions about
    whether they should behave
  • 3:14 - 3:16
    in the short term
    or behave in the long term.
  • 3:16 - 3:19
    So, if they're in an environment
  • 3:19 - 3:22
    in which long term gain
    is very rare,
  • 3:22 - 3:25
    well then, it makes perfect sense
    for them to behave impulsively
  • 3:25 - 3:27
    because that's going to maximize
    their reward.
  • 3:27 - 3:31
    -How's it taste?
    -Mmmm!
  • 3:31 - 3:33
    (narrator) A production
    of the University of Rochester.
  • 3:33 - 3:36
    Please visit us online
    and subscribe to our channel
  • 3:36 - 3:39
    for more videos.
  • 3:39 - 3:44
    [school bell rings]
Title:
Re-evaluating The Marshmallow Test
Description:

more » « less
Video Language:
English
Duration:
03:45

English subtitles

Revisions