< Return to Video

How words change minds: the science of storytelling | Nat Kendall-Taylor | TEDxMidAtlanticSalon

  • 0:07 - 0:09
    I want to start with a warning today.
  • 0:11 - 0:15
    I realize that beginning with a warning
    may not be the smartest thing to do,
  • 0:15 - 0:17
    but I'm just going to go for it.
  • 0:17 - 0:21
    My warning is that although I'm here
    to talk to you about communications,
  • 0:21 - 0:25
    I actually have zero training
    in communications,
  • 0:25 - 0:27
    and I've never worked in PR.
  • 0:27 - 0:28
    What I am
  • 0:28 - 0:31
    is a psychological anthropologist.
  • 0:31 - 0:35
    And what I study is the way
    that culture influences how we think,
  • 0:36 - 0:40
    how we process information,
    how we make meaning of messages,
  • 0:40 - 0:44
    and how we formulate
    and come to decisions.
  • 0:44 - 0:49
    And so, as a psychological anthropologist,
    one of my goals here today
  • 0:49 - 0:52
    is to convince you,
    is to show you, that this ...
  • 0:53 - 0:55
    is not true.
  • 0:55 - 0:56
    (Laughter)
  • 0:56 - 1:00
    That we should not be actively
    dissuading our friends and colleagues
  • 1:00 - 1:02
    from going into anthropology,
  • 1:02 - 1:07
    and that instead, studying culture
    and how people use it to think
  • 1:07 - 1:10
    is an incredibly valuable tool
    in the real world,
  • 1:10 - 1:12
    and for our purposes today,
  • 1:12 - 1:16
    can be an incredibly important
    and effective thing
  • 1:16 - 1:19
    in being a better communicator.
  • 1:19 - 1:23
    And so, as an anthropologist
    working in communications,
  • 1:24 - 1:26
    I study two different things.
  • 1:26 - 1:29
    First of all, I study public thinking,
  • 1:29 - 1:30
    not public opinion,
  • 1:30 - 1:34
    not the way that people answer a couple
    of polling questions here or there,
  • 1:34 - 1:36
    or conduct themselves
    in a few focus groups
  • 1:36 - 1:38
    in Cleveland or Kansas City -
  • 1:38 - 1:41
    I'm from Cleveland,
    I can make that joke, that's okay -
  • 1:41 - 1:46
    but rather how people use culture
    in a deep and highly predictable way
  • 1:46 - 1:50
    to think about complex social issues;
    issues like education or mental health,
  • 1:50 - 1:56
    immigration or aging,
    climate change or race inequity.
  • 1:56 - 2:00
    So I am really excited
    to talk to you about ...
  • 2:01 - 2:05
    how culture helps us
    be more effective communicators.
  • 2:06 - 2:08
    The other thing that I'm going
    to talk to you about
  • 2:08 - 2:11
    is how through the way
    that we present information
  • 2:11 - 2:16
    we can get people to open up
    an access dramatically different ways
  • 2:16 - 2:21
    of thinking, of feeling, and of acting
    about those social issues,
  • 2:21 - 2:25
    and in a nutshell,
    that is what framing is:
  • 2:25 - 2:28
    how variations in the way
    that we present information
  • 2:28 - 2:34
    can lead people to dramatically different
    perceptual and behavioral outcomes.
  • 2:34 - 2:37
    And so I'm really, really - why not? -
  • 2:37 - 2:42
    really excited to get the chance
    to geek out about framing today.
  • 2:42 - 2:44
    And I'll tell you
    right from the beginning
  • 2:44 - 2:48
    that geeking out about framing
    is pretty much my all-time,
  • 2:48 - 2:49
    absolute favorite thing to do,
  • 2:50 - 2:54
    which I realize is kind of pathetic,
    and probably a little bit sad.
  • 2:54 - 2:58
    But it does mean that at least
    one person in this room
  • 2:58 - 3:00
    is going to have fun during this talk.
  • 3:00 - 3:01
    (Laughter)
  • 3:01 - 3:03
    That will be me, I will have fun.
  • 3:04 - 3:07
    And so, what I want to do today
    is to convince you, is to argue
  • 3:07 - 3:10
    that even though you do not
    think of yourselves
  • 3:10 - 3:12
    all the time in this way
  • 3:12 - 3:16
    and are not explicitly aware of it,
    you are all communicators.
  • 3:17 - 3:20
    And as communicators,
    framing matters a great deal to you.
  • 3:20 - 3:26
    So what I want to do is give you
    two reasons why framing matters to you.
  • 3:26 - 3:30
    And the first reason is,
    unfortunately, I'm in the position
  • 3:30 - 3:33
    where I have to tell you
    that you all have a problem.
  • 3:33 - 3:36
    And you should know there aren't
    11 more steps after this,
  • 3:36 - 3:39
    it's not that kind of a meeting,
    and it's not that kind of a problem.
  • 3:39 - 3:42
    What I mean is that you have
    a communications problem.
  • 3:42 - 3:44
    You have a problem of perception.
  • 3:44 - 3:47
    And the problem looks something like this:
  • 3:47 - 3:50
    That you all have been in positions,
    at one time or another,
  • 3:50 - 3:54
    where you think you have the most perfect,
  • 3:54 - 3:56
    awesome, slam-dunk -
  • 3:56 - 3:58
    whatever sports metaphor you want to use -
  • 3:58 - 4:01
    way of talking about
    what you do and why it matters.
  • 4:01 - 4:04
    Heck, it works with two of your
    closest colleagues,
  • 4:04 - 4:07
    what could go wrong
    when it goes out to normal people,
  • 4:07 - 4:11
    people who don't eat and breathe
    and sleep your issues all the time?
  • 4:11 - 4:14
    And you find that when this idea
    that made so much sense to you
  • 4:14 - 4:17
    goes outside of your immediate circle,
    it does one of two things.
  • 4:17 - 4:19
    First of all, it lacks resonance.
  • 4:19 - 4:23
    It doesn't have grip,
    it goes in one ear and out the other.
  • 4:23 - 4:26
    Secondly, probably more unfortunately
    because it happens more frequently,
  • 4:26 - 4:31
    that thing which worked
    and was so brilliant in your own head
  • 4:31 - 4:32
    goes out,
  • 4:32 - 4:37
    and it has the exact opposite effect
    on the people you're trying to persuade,
  • 4:37 - 4:39
    on the people you're trying
    to communicate with.
  • 4:39 - 4:44
    And I'm not going to ask you to take
    my word for anything today, right?
  • 4:44 - 4:48
    I'm going to show you evidence
    from the research that I do with my team
  • 4:48 - 4:49
    that shows this.
  • 4:49 - 4:52
    And I have a lot of pieces of examples,
  • 4:52 - 4:56
    evidence of this you-say-they-think,
    this lost-in-translation effect.
  • 4:56 - 4:59
    I'm going to show you one today
    that comes from some work that we've done
  • 4:59 - 5:03
    to translate the science
    of early childhood development.
  • 5:03 - 5:08
    People who are in this field,
    people who are developmental scientists,
  • 5:08 - 5:11
    really want to talk
    about adversity and stress,
  • 5:11 - 5:14
    and the effects that stress and adversity
    can have on young kids.
  • 5:14 - 5:16
    And they say things like this:
  • 5:16 - 5:18
    that persistent stress
    can derail development
  • 5:18 - 5:21
    and have negative long-term effects
    on health and well-being.
  • 5:21 - 5:25
    And if you're a developmental scientist,
    you replace negative with deleterious
  • 5:25 - 5:27
    because that's the way you talk.
  • 5:27 - 5:29
    And so for folks who are in this field,
  • 5:29 - 5:31
    this is true.
  • 5:31 - 5:35
    There is an incredibly
    deep body of science
  • 5:35 - 5:38
    across a number of disciplines
    which supports this point.
  • 5:38 - 5:42
    Unfortunately,
    when you take this idea out,
  • 5:42 - 5:44
    to normal people,
    to members of the general public,
  • 5:44 - 5:47
    you get things
    that look and sound like this:
  • 5:47 - 5:50
    (Video) Man:
    Life's hard. Supposed to be hard.
  • 5:50 - 5:53
    What doesn't kill you makes
    you stronger, you know?
  • 5:53 - 5:56
    All the bad cliches you can think of.
  • 5:56 - 6:02
    There's been people that have come
    from absolutely nothing to make it,
  • 6:02 - 6:06
    and in society's eyes gained success.
  • 6:06 - 6:09
    Nat Kendall-Taylor:
    So just to make it really crystal-clear,
  • 6:10 - 6:13
    that which you just heard
    was not the intended effect
  • 6:13 - 6:16
    when this expert opened
    his or her mouth to deliver this message.
  • 6:16 - 6:19
    I'm not trying to say
    that our friend Nietzsche here
  • 6:19 - 6:22
    with "what doesn't kill you
    makes you stronger" -
  • 6:22 - 6:24
    smart audience, there you go -
  • 6:24 - 6:26
    is wrong or stupid in any way.
  • 6:26 - 6:29
    But there's clearly something
    that's going on here,
  • 6:29 - 6:30
    there's clearly a difference
  • 6:30 - 6:33
    between the intention
    and the delivery of the message,
  • 6:33 - 6:35
    and it's actual perception and effect.
  • 6:35 - 6:38
    And you all should have a good idea
    as to what that is,
  • 6:38 - 6:41
    based on how I introduced myself
    as an anthropologist.
  • 6:41 - 6:45
    So the thing that stands between
    the you-say and the they-think here is ...
  • 6:46 - 6:47
    culture.
  • 6:48 - 6:51
    Not the external, Indiana Jones
    artifact kind of culture,
  • 6:51 - 6:53
    but rather culture in mind,
  • 6:53 - 6:56
    culture as a set
    of shared patterns of thinking,
  • 6:56 - 7:00
    as a set of shared assumptions
    and propositions that we have
  • 7:00 - 7:02
    and carry around with us in our minds,
  • 7:02 - 7:06
    and use every time
    that we are presented with information,
  • 7:06 - 7:08
    every time that we engage with an issue.
  • 7:08 - 7:10
    And so, what this does,
  • 7:10 - 7:15
    this realization that culture
    is always mediating our meaning
  • 7:15 - 7:19
    and complicating our job
    as communicators, is it gives us -
  • 7:19 - 7:22
    so this is both, kind of
    one of these paradoxical things
  • 7:22 - 7:26
    that's both utterly common sense,
    and completely game-changing -
  • 7:26 - 7:28
    is that this gives us
    a really different way
  • 7:28 - 7:30
    of looking at what has been
  • 7:30 - 7:34
    the dominant way of thinking about
    public understanding and communication.
  • 7:34 - 7:36
    So for a long time,
    and still too this day,
  • 7:36 - 7:39
    people have thought
    of public understanding in this way:
  • 7:39 - 7:44
    as an empty receptacle,
    as a blank slate, as an empty fishbowl,
  • 7:44 - 7:48
    and have thought that we as communicators
    can assume that we are our audiences,
  • 7:48 - 7:51
    and take the things
    that make so much sense to us,
  • 7:51 - 7:53
    and literally drop them
    into this unfettered space
  • 7:53 - 7:55
    where they get to do their thing.
  • 7:55 - 7:58
    And we know, based on
    what I've just told you about culture,
  • 7:58 - 8:00
    that this is neither correct,
  • 8:00 - 8:04
    nor is it productive as a way
    to think about communications.
  • 8:04 - 8:05
    Instead,
  • 8:05 - 8:09
    we have to understand that culture always
    complicates our job as communicators,
  • 8:09 - 8:11
    and if we can go a step further
  • 8:11 - 8:15
    and understand how people use culture
    to think about our issues,
  • 8:15 - 8:17
    we can be dramatically more effective
  • 8:17 - 8:21
    in our roles as messengers,
    in our roles as communicators.
  • 8:22 - 8:27
    And so, the second reason why framing
    matters to all of you in this room
  • 8:27 - 8:31
    is because understanding
    is frame dependent.
  • 8:32 - 8:35
    Now, that's a mildly
    academicese way of saying
  • 8:35 - 8:39
    that the choices that you make
    as communicators matter.
  • 8:39 - 8:40
    Sometimes the little things:
  • 8:40 - 8:43
    the pronouns that you use,
    the verbs that you choose;
  • 8:43 - 8:44
    sometimes the big things,
  • 8:44 - 8:48
    the values that you use
    to explain why your issue matters;
  • 8:48 - 8:49
    those things matter.
  • 8:49 - 8:51
    Those things have
    frequently dramatic impacts
  • 8:51 - 8:53
    on what people are willing to do,
  • 8:53 - 8:56
    and how people are willing to act
    and engage on your issues.
  • 8:56 - 8:59
    And again, I don't want you
    to take my word for it.
  • 8:59 - 9:01
    I'm going to give you a quick example
  • 9:01 - 9:03
    that shows you that understanding
    is frame dependent.
  • 9:03 - 9:06
    And this example comes
    not from the United States,
  • 9:06 - 9:09
    but from the Canadian province of Alberta.
  • 9:09 - 9:11
    And a quick geography lesson,
  • 9:11 - 9:14
    Alberta is one of the tall, skinny ones
    in the middle of the country.
  • 9:14 - 9:18
    Kind of all you need to know -
    it's very cold - for this example.
  • 9:18 - 9:21
    And so there's a group of experts
    and advocates in Alberta
  • 9:21 - 9:25
    who are working to change
    policy and practice around addiction.
  • 9:25 - 9:27
    They're working to take
    what we know from science,
  • 9:27 - 9:30
    and use it to implement better policies
    and practices around addiction
  • 9:30 - 9:32
    in this province.
  • 9:32 - 9:34
    And they've been having
    a great deal of difficulty doing this.
  • 9:35 - 9:37
    A lot of their problem comes from the fact
  • 9:37 - 9:40
    that there is zero support
    to do anything different
  • 9:40 - 9:42
    when it comes to addiction
    in this province.
  • 9:42 - 9:45
    And so, they came to us,
    and they asked us to conduct some work
  • 9:45 - 9:48
    to figure out how to engage
    members of the public more productively,
  • 9:48 - 9:49
    to move understanding,
  • 9:49 - 9:53
    and specifically, to increase support
    for a set of evidence-based policies.
  • 9:53 - 9:56
    And so, as good framing geeks and dweebs,
  • 9:56 - 9:58
    we do what good framing geeks
    and dweebs do,
  • 9:58 - 10:00
    we ran an experiment.
  • 10:00 - 10:05
    And in this experiment
    we tested three different values messages.
  • 10:05 - 10:09
    You see, the values messages
    along the horizontal axis of this graph
  • 10:09 - 10:10
    right now.
  • 10:10 - 10:12
    So some people -
    this is a large experiment,
  • 10:12 - 10:16
    6,000 people, which believe it or not
    is not the entire population of Alberta,
  • 10:17 - 10:20
    it's a representative sample,
    not an exhaustive sample.
  • 10:20 - 10:25
    Each of these 6,000 people is randomly
    assigned to one of these messages.
  • 10:25 - 10:27
    So if some folks got the value
    of interdependence,
  • 10:27 - 10:29
    which, in this case, is the sense
  • 10:29 - 10:32
    that we need to do a better job
    of dealing with addiction in this province
  • 10:32 - 10:34
    because we're all connected:
  • 10:34 - 10:36
    what influences one of us
    influences all of us.
  • 10:36 - 10:39
    Other folks got this value of ingenuity,
  • 10:39 - 10:43
    which is an innovation value, that we are
    a province of problem-solvers -
  • 10:43 - 10:45
    you kind of swing your arm
    when you do this one -
  • 10:45 - 10:48
    there's never been a problem
    that we haven't been able to solve
  • 10:48 - 10:50
    with some good old Albertan grit
  • 10:50 - 10:52
    and roll-up-your-sleeves
    problem-solvingness -
  • 10:52 - 10:56
    that was my Albertan accent,
    if you caught that, very important.
  • 10:56 - 10:59
    And other folks, last but not least,
    got this value of empathy,
  • 10:59 - 11:01
    which is the sense that we need
    to do a better job
  • 11:01 - 11:03
    of dealing with addiction in this province
  • 11:03 - 11:06
    because people who deal with
    addiction are people too.
  • 11:06 - 11:09
    They could be our mother, brother,
    father, sister, neighbor, whomever,
  • 11:09 - 11:12
    and as individuals, we need to show
    these folks compassion.
  • 11:12 - 11:15
    So what you're going to see
    on this next click
  • 11:15 - 11:19
    is what I think are three beautiful,
    blue bars appearing on this screen,
  • 11:19 - 11:21
    and what those blue bars
    are going to show you
  • 11:21 - 11:25
    is the extent, the degree
    to which hearing these different values
  • 11:25 - 11:28
    changes people's support
    for these evidence-based policies.
  • 11:28 - 11:30
    So can anyone do a good drumroll?
  • 11:30 - 11:33
    Please, play along, thank you.
  • 11:33 - 11:35
    (Drumroll)
  • 11:35 - 11:39
    So you should see three blue bars
    and notice two things.
  • 11:39 - 11:42
    So first of all, two of these values,
    interdependence and ingenuity,
  • 11:42 - 11:45
    make people, to a statistically
    significant degree,
  • 11:45 - 11:48
    more supportive of these
    evidence-based policies.
  • 11:48 - 11:51
    That is good news
    when we run these experiments,
  • 11:51 - 11:52
    and when we get results like that,
  • 11:52 - 11:54
    we stand up, we do
    a little framing dance -
  • 11:54 - 11:56
    I won't do it right now, don't worry -
  • 11:56 - 12:00
    we sit back down and we look
    towards the right-hand side of the screen.
  • 12:00 - 12:01
    The value of empathy
  • 12:01 - 12:04
    is actually depressing
    people's support for these policies.
  • 12:05 - 12:08
    Now, the kicker is
    that in a subsequent piece of analysis,
  • 12:08 - 12:12
    where we looked at all of the fields
    external-facing materials,
  • 12:12 - 12:17
    guess which value we found
    to be in place over 90% of the time?
  • 12:18 - 12:18
    Empathy.
  • 12:18 - 12:20
    Thank you. Not a rhetorical question.
  • 12:20 - 12:24
    And so, what this field has been doing
    for a very long time is endorsing a value
  • 12:24 - 12:26
    which actually drives support down
  • 12:26 - 12:28
    for the very policies
    that they are advocating.
  • 12:28 - 12:31
    So this example does two things:
  • 12:31 - 12:34
    it clearly shows you that understanding
    is frame-dependent and frames matter.
  • 12:34 - 12:38
    It also shows you that these questions,
    you know, which values to use,
  • 12:38 - 12:40
    how to communicate,
    are empirical questions.
  • 12:40 - 12:44
    We don't have to guess or use our guts,
    we can use social science.
  • 12:45 - 12:46
    I think it's pretty cool
  • 12:46 - 12:50
    that frames are able to move people's
    understanding and their policy support,
  • 12:50 - 12:54
    but what about more intrinsic,
    subconscious thinking?
  • 12:54 - 12:56
    What about implicit bias?
  • 12:56 - 13:00
    Can frames make people
    less subconsciously biased
  • 13:00 - 13:02
    against particular groups of people?
  • 13:02 - 13:06
    So we set out to answer this question
    through a project on re-framing aging
  • 13:06 - 13:09
    in which we were
    specifically interested in:
  • 13:09 - 13:14
    can frames make people less
    implicitly biased against older adults?
  • 13:14 - 13:15
    And we found two things.
  • 13:15 - 13:20
    First of all, Americans do not
    like older people.
  • 13:21 - 13:24
    Older Americans don't like older people.
  • 13:24 - 13:25
    (Laughter)
  • 13:25 - 13:26
    High degree of implicit bias,
  • 13:26 - 13:28
    and it's a level of implicit bias
  • 13:28 - 13:30
    that parallels other biases
    that people study,
  • 13:30 - 13:35
    whether that's gender,
    religion, sexuality, race;
  • 13:35 - 13:38
    this is not cool news, not a good finding.
  • 13:38 - 13:40
    But it does get cool
    when you look at what happens
  • 13:40 - 13:42
    when we gave people a message
  • 13:42 - 13:46
    that compared ageing to a process
    of building and gaining momentum.
  • 13:46 - 13:47
    And when we did this,
  • 13:47 - 13:52
    we found that we could actually reduce
    people's implicit bias by almost a third.
  • 13:52 - 13:54
    Through a frame,
  • 13:54 - 13:58
    we could make people less ageist
    at an implicit level.
  • 13:58 - 14:01
    And you can tell
    that I think this is pretty cool,
  • 14:01 - 14:04
    and it's definitely evidence
    that frames matter,
  • 14:04 - 14:08
    and it's definitely evidence
    that understanding is frame dependent.
  • 14:08 - 14:13
    So I want to leave you with a quote,
    one of my new favorite quotes.
  • 14:13 - 14:16
    This is from Austrian
    philosopher Ivan Illich,
  • 14:16 - 14:18
    and Illich says that neither
    revolution nor reformation
  • 14:18 - 14:22
    can ultimately change a society,
    rather you must tell a more powerful tale,
  • 14:22 - 14:25
    one so persuasive
    that it sweeps away the old myths
  • 14:25 - 14:28
    and becomes the preferred story.
  • 14:28 - 14:33
    So if we're going to drive social change,
    we need to develop, we need to test,
  • 14:33 - 14:37
    and we need to commit to
    telling new stories.
  • 14:37 - 14:42
    And with that, I will thank you very much,
    and encourage you all to frame on.
  • 14:42 - 14:45
    (Applause)
Title:
How words change minds: the science of storytelling | Nat Kendall-Taylor | TEDxMidAtlanticSalon
Description:

Sometimes a good idea isn't enough to change minds or garner support – more important is how you articulate that idea. This is where the science of "framing" comes in. In his talk, Nat Kendall-Taylor breaks down how people make decisions, and how understanding culture and behavioral science can be used to communicate complex issues and shape policy.

Nat Kendall-Taylor is Chief Executive Officer at the FrameWorks Institute. Nat oversees the organization’s pioneering, research-based approach to strategic communications, which uses methods from the social and behavioral sciences to measure how people understand complex socio-political issues and tests ways to reframe them to drive social change. As CEO, he leads a multi-disciplinary team of social scientists and communications practitioners who investigate ways to apply innovative framing research methods to social issues and train nonprofit organizations to put the findings into practice.

This talk was given at a TEDx event using the TED conference format but independently organized by a local community. Learn more at https://www.ted.com/tedx

more » « less
Video Language:
English
Team:
closed TED
Project:
TEDxTalks
Duration:
14:48

English subtitles

Revisions