How words change minds: the science of storytelling | Nat Kendall-Taylor | TEDxMidAtlanticSalon
-
0:07 - 0:09I want to start with a warning today.
-
0:11 - 0:15I realize that beginning with a warning
may not be the smartest thing to do, -
0:15 - 0:17but I'm just going to go for it.
-
0:17 - 0:21My warning is that although I'm here
to talk to you about communications, -
0:21 - 0:25I actually have zero training
in communications, -
0:25 - 0:27and I've never worked in PR.
-
0:27 - 0:28What I am
-
0:28 - 0:31is a psychological anthropologist.
-
0:31 - 0:35And what I study is the way
that culture influences how we think, -
0:36 - 0:40how we process information,
how we make meaning of messages, -
0:40 - 0:44and how we formulate
and come to decisions. -
0:44 - 0:49And so, as a psychological anthropologist,
one of my goals here today -
0:49 - 0:52is to convince you,
is to show you, that this ... -
0:53 - 0:55is not true.
-
0:55 - 0:56(Laughter)
-
0:56 - 1:00That we should not be actively
dissuading our friends and colleagues -
1:00 - 1:02from going into anthropology,
-
1:02 - 1:07and that instead, studying culture
and how people use it to think -
1:07 - 1:10is an incredibly valuable tool
in the real world, -
1:10 - 1:12and for our purposes today,
-
1:12 - 1:16can be an incredibly important
and effective thing -
1:16 - 1:19in being a better communicator.
-
1:19 - 1:23And so, as an anthropologist
working in communications, -
1:24 - 1:26I study two different things.
-
1:26 - 1:29First of all, I study public thinking,
-
1:29 - 1:30not public opinion,
-
1:30 - 1:34not the way that people answer a couple
of polling questions here or there, -
1:34 - 1:36or conduct themselves
in a few focus groups -
1:36 - 1:38in Cleveland or Kansas City -
-
1:38 - 1:41I'm from Cleveland,
I can make that joke, that's okay - -
1:41 - 1:46but rather how people use culture
in a deep and highly predictable way -
1:46 - 1:50to think about complex social issues;
issues like education or mental health, -
1:50 - 1:56immigration or aging,
climate change or race inequity. -
1:56 - 2:00So I am really excited
to talk to you about ... -
2:01 - 2:05how culture helps us
be more effective communicators. -
2:06 - 2:08The other thing that I'm going
to talk to you about -
2:08 - 2:11is how through the way
that we present information -
2:11 - 2:16we can get people to open up
an access dramatically different ways -
2:16 - 2:21of thinking, of feeling, and of acting
about those social issues, -
2:21 - 2:25and in a nutshell,
that is what framing is: -
2:25 - 2:28how variations in the way
that we present information -
2:28 - 2:34can lead people to dramatically different
perceptual and behavioral outcomes. -
2:34 - 2:37And so I'm really, really - why not? -
-
2:37 - 2:42really excited to get the chance
to geek out about framing today. -
2:42 - 2:44And I'll tell you
right from the beginning -
2:44 - 2:48that geeking out about framing
is pretty much my all-time, -
2:48 - 2:49absolute favorite thing to do,
-
2:50 - 2:54which I realize is kind of pathetic,
and probably a little bit sad. -
2:54 - 2:58But it does mean that at least
one person in this room -
2:58 - 3:00is going to have fun during this talk.
-
3:00 - 3:01(Laughter)
-
3:01 - 3:03That will be me, I will have fun.
-
3:04 - 3:07And so, what I want to do today
is to convince you, is to argue -
3:07 - 3:10that even though you do not
think of yourselves -
3:10 - 3:12all the time in this way
-
3:12 - 3:16and are not explicitly aware of it,
you are all communicators. -
3:17 - 3:20And as communicators,
framing matters a great deal to you. -
3:20 - 3:26So what I want to do is give you
two reasons why framing matters to you. -
3:26 - 3:30And the first reason is,
unfortunately, I'm in the position -
3:30 - 3:33where I have to tell you
that you all have a problem. -
3:33 - 3:36And you should know there aren't
11 more steps after this, -
3:36 - 3:39it's not that kind of a meeting,
and it's not that kind of a problem. -
3:39 - 3:42What I mean is that you have
a communications problem. -
3:42 - 3:44You have a problem of perception.
-
3:44 - 3:47And the problem looks something like this:
-
3:47 - 3:50That you all have been in positions,
at one time or another, -
3:50 - 3:54where you think you have the most perfect,
-
3:54 - 3:56awesome, slam-dunk -
-
3:56 - 3:58whatever sports metaphor you want to use -
-
3:58 - 4:01way of talking about
what you do and why it matters. -
4:01 - 4:04Heck, it works with two of your
closest colleagues, -
4:04 - 4:07what could go wrong
when it goes out to normal people, -
4:07 - 4:11people who don't eat and breathe
and sleep your issues all the time? -
4:11 - 4:14And you find that when this idea
that made so much sense to you -
4:14 - 4:17goes outside of your immediate circle,
it does one of two things. -
4:17 - 4:19First of all, it lacks resonance.
-
4:19 - 4:23It doesn't have grip,
it goes in one ear and out the other. -
4:23 - 4:26Secondly, probably more unfortunately
because it happens more frequently, -
4:26 - 4:31that thing which worked
and was so brilliant in your own head -
4:31 - 4:32goes out,
-
4:32 - 4:37and it has the exact opposite effect
on the people you're trying to persuade, -
4:37 - 4:39on the people you're trying
to communicate with. -
4:39 - 4:44And I'm not going to ask you to take
my word for anything today, right? -
4:44 - 4:48I'm going to show you evidence
from the research that I do with my team -
4:48 - 4:49that shows this.
-
4:49 - 4:52And I have a lot of pieces of examples,
-
4:52 - 4:56evidence of this you-say-they-think,
this lost-in-translation effect. -
4:56 - 4:59I'm going to show you one today
that comes from some work that we've done -
4:59 - 5:03to translate the science
of early childhood development. -
5:03 - 5:08People who are in this field,
people who are developmental scientists, -
5:08 - 5:11really want to talk
about adversity and stress, -
5:11 - 5:14and the effects that stress and adversity
can have on young kids. -
5:14 - 5:16And they say things like this:
-
5:16 - 5:18that persistent stress
can derail development -
5:18 - 5:21and have negative long-term effects
on health and well-being. -
5:21 - 5:25And if you're a developmental scientist,
you replace negative with deleterious -
5:25 - 5:27because that's the way you talk.
-
5:27 - 5:29And so for folks who are in this field,
-
5:29 - 5:31this is true.
-
5:31 - 5:35There is an incredibly
deep body of science -
5:35 - 5:38across a number of disciplines
which supports this point. -
5:38 - 5:42Unfortunately,
when you take this idea out, -
5:42 - 5:44to normal people,
to members of the general public, -
5:44 - 5:47you get things
that look and sound like this: -
5:47 - 5:50(Video) Man:
Life's hard. Supposed to be hard. -
5:50 - 5:53What doesn't kill you makes
you stronger, you know? -
5:53 - 5:56All the bad cliches you can think of.
-
5:56 - 6:02There's been people that have come
from absolutely nothing to make it, -
6:02 - 6:06and in society's eyes gained success.
-
6:06 - 6:09Nat Kendall-Taylor:
So just to make it really crystal-clear, -
6:10 - 6:13that which you just heard
was not the intended effect -
6:13 - 6:16when this expert opened
his or her mouth to deliver this message. -
6:16 - 6:19I'm not trying to say
that our friend Nietzsche here -
6:19 - 6:22with "what doesn't kill you
makes you stronger" - -
6:22 - 6:24smart audience, there you go -
-
6:24 - 6:26is wrong or stupid in any way.
-
6:26 - 6:29But there's clearly something
that's going on here, -
6:29 - 6:30there's clearly a difference
-
6:30 - 6:33between the intention
and the delivery of the message, -
6:33 - 6:35and it's actual perception and effect.
-
6:35 - 6:38And you all should have a good idea
as to what that is, -
6:38 - 6:41based on how I introduced myself
as an anthropologist. -
6:41 - 6:45So the thing that stands between
the you-say and the they-think here is ... -
6:46 - 6:47culture.
-
6:48 - 6:51Not the external, Indiana Jones
artifact kind of culture, -
6:51 - 6:53but rather culture in mind,
-
6:53 - 6:56culture as a set
of shared patterns of thinking, -
6:56 - 7:00as a set of shared assumptions
and propositions that we have -
7:00 - 7:02and carry around with us in our minds,
-
7:02 - 7:06and use every time
that we are presented with information, -
7:06 - 7:08every time that we engage with an issue.
-
7:08 - 7:10And so, what this does,
-
7:10 - 7:15this realization that culture
is always mediating our meaning -
7:15 - 7:19and complicating our job
as communicators, is it gives us - -
7:19 - 7:22so this is both, kind of
one of these paradoxical things -
7:22 - 7:26that's both utterly common sense,
and completely game-changing - -
7:26 - 7:28is that this gives us
a really different way -
7:28 - 7:30of looking at what has been
-
7:30 - 7:34the dominant way of thinking about
public understanding and communication. -
7:34 - 7:36So for a long time,
and still too this day, -
7:36 - 7:39people have thought
of public understanding in this way: -
7:39 - 7:44as an empty receptacle,
as a blank slate, as an empty fishbowl, -
7:44 - 7:48and have thought that we as communicators
can assume that we are our audiences, -
7:48 - 7:51and take the things
that make so much sense to us, -
7:51 - 7:53and literally drop them
into this unfettered space -
7:53 - 7:55where they get to do their thing.
-
7:55 - 7:58And we know, based on
what I've just told you about culture, -
7:58 - 8:00that this is neither correct,
-
8:00 - 8:04nor is it productive as a way
to think about communications. -
8:04 - 8:05Instead,
-
8:05 - 8:09we have to understand that culture always
complicates our job as communicators, -
8:09 - 8:11and if we can go a step further
-
8:11 - 8:15and understand how people use culture
to think about our issues, -
8:15 - 8:17we can be dramatically more effective
-
8:17 - 8:21in our roles as messengers,
in our roles as communicators. -
8:22 - 8:27And so, the second reason why framing
matters to all of you in this room -
8:27 - 8:31is because understanding
is frame dependent. -
8:32 - 8:35Now, that's a mildly
academicese way of saying -
8:35 - 8:39that the choices that you make
as communicators matter. -
8:39 - 8:40Sometimes the little things:
-
8:40 - 8:43the pronouns that you use,
the verbs that you choose; -
8:43 - 8:44sometimes the big things,
-
8:44 - 8:48the values that you use
to explain why your issue matters; -
8:48 - 8:49those things matter.
-
8:49 - 8:51Those things have
frequently dramatic impacts -
8:51 - 8:53on what people are willing to do,
-
8:53 - 8:56and how people are willing to act
and engage on your issues. -
8:56 - 8:59And again, I don't want you
to take my word for it. -
8:59 - 9:01I'm going to give you a quick example
-
9:01 - 9:03that shows you that understanding
is frame dependent. -
9:03 - 9:06And this example comes
not from the United States, -
9:06 - 9:09but from the Canadian province of Alberta.
-
9:09 - 9:11And a quick geography lesson,
-
9:11 - 9:14Alberta is one of the tall, skinny ones
in the middle of the country. -
9:14 - 9:18Kind of all you need to know -
it's very cold - for this example. -
9:18 - 9:21And so there's a group of experts
and advocates in Alberta -
9:21 - 9:25who are working to change
policy and practice around addiction. -
9:25 - 9:27They're working to take
what we know from science, -
9:27 - 9:30and use it to implement better policies
and practices around addiction -
9:30 - 9:32in this province.
-
9:32 - 9:34And they've been having
a great deal of difficulty doing this. -
9:35 - 9:37A lot of their problem comes from the fact
-
9:37 - 9:40that there is zero support
to do anything different -
9:40 - 9:42when it comes to addiction
in this province. -
9:42 - 9:45And so, they came to us,
and they asked us to conduct some work -
9:45 - 9:48to figure out how to engage
members of the public more productively, -
9:48 - 9:49to move understanding,
-
9:49 - 9:53and specifically, to increase support
for a set of evidence-based policies. -
9:53 - 9:56And so, as good framing geeks and dweebs,
-
9:56 - 9:58we do what good framing geeks
and dweebs do, -
9:58 - 10:00we ran an experiment.
-
10:00 - 10:05And in this experiment
we tested three different values messages. -
10:05 - 10:09You see, the values messages
along the horizontal axis of this graph -
10:09 - 10:10right now.
-
10:10 - 10:12So some people -
this is a large experiment, -
10:12 - 10:166,000 people, which believe it or not
is not the entire population of Alberta, -
10:17 - 10:20it's a representative sample,
not an exhaustive sample. -
10:20 - 10:25Each of these 6,000 people is randomly
assigned to one of these messages. -
10:25 - 10:27So if some folks got the value
of interdependence, -
10:27 - 10:29which, in this case, is the sense
-
10:29 - 10:32that we need to do a better job
of dealing with addiction in this province -
10:32 - 10:34because we're all connected:
-
10:34 - 10:36what influences one of us
influences all of us. -
10:36 - 10:39Other folks got this value of ingenuity,
-
10:39 - 10:43which is an innovation value, that we are
a province of problem-solvers - -
10:43 - 10:45you kind of swing your arm
when you do this one - -
10:45 - 10:48there's never been a problem
that we haven't been able to solve -
10:48 - 10:50with some good old Albertan grit
-
10:50 - 10:52and roll-up-your-sleeves
problem-solvingness - -
10:52 - 10:56that was my Albertan accent,
if you caught that, very important. -
10:56 - 10:59And other folks, last but not least,
got this value of empathy, -
10:59 - 11:01which is the sense that we need
to do a better job -
11:01 - 11:03of dealing with addiction in this province
-
11:03 - 11:06because people who deal with
addiction are people too. -
11:06 - 11:09They could be our mother, brother,
father, sister, neighbor, whomever, -
11:09 - 11:12and as individuals, we need to show
these folks compassion. -
11:12 - 11:15So what you're going to see
on this next click -
11:15 - 11:19is what I think are three beautiful,
blue bars appearing on this screen, -
11:19 - 11:21and what those blue bars
are going to show you -
11:21 - 11:25is the extent, the degree
to which hearing these different values -
11:25 - 11:28changes people's support
for these evidence-based policies. -
11:28 - 11:30So can anyone do a good drumroll?
-
11:30 - 11:33Please, play along, thank you.
-
11:33 - 11:35(Drumroll)
-
11:35 - 11:39So you should see three blue bars
and notice two things. -
11:39 - 11:42So first of all, two of these values,
interdependence and ingenuity, -
11:42 - 11:45make people, to a statistically
significant degree, -
11:45 - 11:48more supportive of these
evidence-based policies. -
11:48 - 11:51That is good news
when we run these experiments, -
11:51 - 11:52and when we get results like that,
-
11:52 - 11:54we stand up, we do
a little framing dance - -
11:54 - 11:56I won't do it right now, don't worry -
-
11:56 - 12:00we sit back down and we look
towards the right-hand side of the screen. -
12:00 - 12:01The value of empathy
-
12:01 - 12:04is actually depressing
people's support for these policies. -
12:05 - 12:08Now, the kicker is
that in a subsequent piece of analysis, -
12:08 - 12:12where we looked at all of the fields
external-facing materials, -
12:12 - 12:17guess which value we found
to be in place over 90% of the time? -
12:18 - 12:18Empathy.
-
12:18 - 12:20Thank you. Not a rhetorical question.
-
12:20 - 12:24And so, what this field has been doing
for a very long time is endorsing a value -
12:24 - 12:26which actually drives support down
-
12:26 - 12:28for the very policies
that they are advocating. -
12:28 - 12:31So this example does two things:
-
12:31 - 12:34it clearly shows you that understanding
is frame-dependent and frames matter. -
12:34 - 12:38It also shows you that these questions,
you know, which values to use, -
12:38 - 12:40how to communicate,
are empirical questions. -
12:40 - 12:44We don't have to guess or use our guts,
we can use social science. -
12:45 - 12:46I think it's pretty cool
-
12:46 - 12:50that frames are able to move people's
understanding and their policy support, -
12:50 - 12:54but what about more intrinsic,
subconscious thinking? -
12:54 - 12:56What about implicit bias?
-
12:56 - 13:00Can frames make people
less subconsciously biased -
13:00 - 13:02against particular groups of people?
-
13:02 - 13:06So we set out to answer this question
through a project on re-framing aging -
13:06 - 13:09in which we were
specifically interested in: -
13:09 - 13:14can frames make people less
implicitly biased against older adults? -
13:14 - 13:15And we found two things.
-
13:15 - 13:20First of all, Americans do not
like older people. -
13:21 - 13:24Older Americans don't like older people.
-
13:24 - 13:25(Laughter)
-
13:25 - 13:26High degree of implicit bias,
-
13:26 - 13:28and it's a level of implicit bias
-
13:28 - 13:30that parallels other biases
that people study, -
13:30 - 13:35whether that's gender,
religion, sexuality, race; -
13:35 - 13:38this is not cool news, not a good finding.
-
13:38 - 13:40But it does get cool
when you look at what happens -
13:40 - 13:42when we gave people a message
-
13:42 - 13:46that compared ageing to a process
of building and gaining momentum. -
13:46 - 13:47And when we did this,
-
13:47 - 13:52we found that we could actually reduce
people's implicit bias by almost a third. -
13:52 - 13:54Through a frame,
-
13:54 - 13:58we could make people less ageist
at an implicit level. -
13:58 - 14:01And you can tell
that I think this is pretty cool, -
14:01 - 14:04and it's definitely evidence
that frames matter, -
14:04 - 14:08and it's definitely evidence
that understanding is frame dependent. -
14:08 - 14:13So I want to leave you with a quote,
one of my new favorite quotes. -
14:13 - 14:16This is from Austrian
philosopher Ivan Illich, -
14:16 - 14:18and Illich says that neither
revolution nor reformation -
14:18 - 14:22can ultimately change a society,
rather you must tell a more powerful tale, -
14:22 - 14:25one so persuasive
that it sweeps away the old myths -
14:25 - 14:28and becomes the preferred story.
-
14:28 - 14:33So if we're going to drive social change,
we need to develop, we need to test, -
14:33 - 14:37and we need to commit to
telling new stories. -
14:37 - 14:42And with that, I will thank you very much,
and encourage you all to frame on. -
14:42 - 14:45(Applause)
- Title:
- How words change minds: the science of storytelling | Nat Kendall-Taylor | TEDxMidAtlanticSalon
- Description:
-
Sometimes a good idea isn't enough to change minds or garner support – more important is how you articulate that idea. This is where the science of "framing" comes in. In his talk, Nat Kendall-Taylor breaks down how people make decisions, and how understanding culture and behavioral science can be used to communicate complex issues and shape policy.
Nat Kendall-Taylor is Chief Executive Officer at the FrameWorks Institute. Nat oversees the organization’s pioneering, research-based approach to strategic communications, which uses methods from the social and behavioral sciences to measure how people understand complex socio-political issues and tests ways to reframe them to drive social change. As CEO, he leads a multi-disciplinary team of social scientists and communications practitioners who investigate ways to apply innovative framing research methods to social issues and train nonprofit organizations to put the findings into practice.
This talk was given at a TEDx event using the TED conference format but independently organized by a local community. Learn more at https://www.ted.com/tedx
- Video Language:
- English
- Team:
closed TED
- Project:
- TEDxTalks
- Duration:
- 14:48